Jeffrey Eric Brigman v. State of Alaska ( 2022 )


Menu:
  •                                            NOTICE
    The text of this opinion can be correct before the opinion is published in the
    Pacific Reporter. Readers are encouraged to bring typographical or other
    formal errors to the attention of the Clerk of the Appellate Courts:
    303 K Street, Anchorage, Alaska 99501
    Fax: (907) 264-0878
    E-mail: corrections@akcourts.gov
    IN THE COURT OF APPEALS OF THE STATE OF ALASKA
    JEFFREY ERIC BRIGMAN,
    Court of Appeals No. A-12727
    Appellant,              Trial Court No. 3AN-13-09635 CR
    v.
    OPINION
    STATE OF ALASKA,
    Appellee.                   No. 2724 — April 22, 2022
    Appeal from the Superior Court, Third Judicial District,
    Anchorage, Kevin M. Saxby, Judge.
    Appearances: Justin N. Gillette, Assistant Public Defender, and
    Samantha Cherot, Public Defender, Anchorage, for the
    Appellant. Eric A. Ringsmuth, Assistant Attorney General,
    Office of Criminal Appeals, Anchorage, and Treg R. Taylor,
    Attorney General, Juneau, for the Appellee.
    Before: Allard, Chief Judge, Harbison, Judge, and Mannheimer,
    Senior Judge.*
    Judge ALLARD.
    *
    Sitting by assignment made pursuant to Article IV, Section 11 of the Alaska
    Constitution and Administrative Rule 23(a).
    Jeffrey Eric Brigman was convicted, following a jury trial, of felony driving
    under the influence.1 In his original appeal, Brigman argued that the superior court erred
    when it failed to suppress an out-of-court eyewitness identification that resulted from a
    showup procedure.2           Because the superior court had not analyzed the showup
    identification under the Alaska Supreme Court’s newly adopted Young standard, we
    remanded Brigman’s case so that the superior court could conduct that analysis.3
    On remand, the superior court found that the showup identification was
    sufficiently reliable under Young and was therefore admissible at trial. Brigman now
    appeals that ruling. For the reasons explained here, we conclude that the superior court
    did not err in its Young analysis.
    Relevant facts
    Around 10:00 p.m. on a September evening in 2013, a woman stepped out
    of a gas station in Anchorage to find that her car had been stolen. A few minutes later,
    the driver of the stolen car crashed into a sidewalk area near 10th Avenue and Juneau
    Street.
    Thomas Jenkins, who was walking his dog on 10th Avenue, witnessed the
    crash, and he approached the driver to offer assistance. Jenkins believed, based on his
    contact with the driver, that the driver was under the influence of something. Jenkins
    1
    AS 28.35.030(n).
    2
    A showup is essentially a single-person lineup. A single suspect is presented to a
    witness to see if the witness can make an identification. These often occur at or near the
    scene of a crime soon after it takes place. See Brigman v. State, 
    2020 WL 704854
     (Alaska
    App. Feb. 12, 2020) (unpublished).
    3
    
    Id.
     (citing Young v. State, 
    374 P.3d 395
     (Alaska 2016)).
    –2–                                        2724
    spoke to the driver briefly before leaving to retrieve his cell phone and call 911.4 The
    driver left the scene before Jenkins returned.
    When Jenkins called 911, he described the driver as a man with black hair,
    gray clothing, and wearing a zippered hoodie. Jenkins thought the man was white, but
    he could not be sure. Jenkins also told the 911 operator that the man might have walked
    south on Ingra Street when he left the scene.
    Almost immediately after this 911 call, Officer Angelina Fraize spotted and
    detained a white male suspect with dark hair and dark clothing walking south on Ingra
    Street, approximately a block from the crash scene. This suspect was Brigman. At trial,
    Officer Fraize described Brigman as “obviously intoxicated,” “sweaty,” and “nervous.”
    Another officer, Steven Childers, brought Jenkins to where Officer Fraize
    was holding Brigman, so that the police could conduct a showup identification — asking
    Jenkins if he could identify Brigman as the driver he had spoken to several minutes
    earlier. To facilitate this showup, Officer Fraize had Brigman stand next to her police
    car in handcuffs.
    Before asking Jenkins whether he could identify the man standing next to
    the patrol car, Officer Childers specifically instructed Jenkins that this man might or
    might not be the right person. Jenkins identified Brigman as the driver of the crashed
    vehicle.
    At trial, Jenkins testified to his identification of Brigman. Jenkins testified
    that Brigman was wearing a gray hoodie at the time of the crash. (Brigman was not
    4
    It was evening when Jenkins observed the driver. Jenkins testified that there was a
    street lamp about twenty-five feet from where the crash occurred that offered some additional
    light by which to see.
    –3–                                          2724
    wearing a hoodie in the photograph taken of him at the scene of the showup
    identification.)
    Another eyewitness to the crash also identified Brigman at trial. This
    second eyewitness testified that he was drunk when he witnessed the crash and that the
    police never asked him to participate in a showup identification or any other out-of-court
    identification. Instead, at trial, this eyewitness was shown the photograph of Brigman
    taken during the showup identification with Jenkins. This second eyewitness identified
    Brigman as the driver of the crashed vehicle, although he stated that he could not be sure.
    The eyewitness had previously described the driver as a white male adult wearing a gray
    shirt with wavy, dark-colored, shoulder-length hair. (Brigman’s hair in the photograph
    is shorter than shoulder-length.)
    Brigman was convicted at trial.
    Brigman’s motion to suppress and our remand to the superior court
    Prior to trial, Brigman moved to suppress the results of the showup
    identification by Jenkins, arguing that the showup procedure was improperly suggestive
    and that the resulting identification was therefore unreliable.
    The superior court held an evidentiary hearing on this motion. After
    evaluating the showup under the then-existing legal test set forth in Anderson v. State
    and Manson v. Braithwaite,5 the court ruled that the result of the showup was admissible.
    However, after the superior court made its ruling, the Alaska Supreme
    Court adopted a new test for evaluating the reliability of out-of-court police identification
    5
    Anderson v. State, 
    123 P.3d 1110
    , 1116 (Alaska App. 2005); Manson v. Braithwaite,
    
    432 U.S. 98
    , 114 (1977).
    –4–                                          2724
    procedures in Young v. State.6 In Brigman’s initial appeal, the State conceded that the
    Young test applied to Brigman’s case, and we therefore remanded the case to the superior
    court to conduct the appropriate analysis under Young.7
    An overview of the Young test
    In Young v. State, the Alaska Supreme Court held that the
    Anderson/Braithwaite test for evaluating the reliability of out-of-court eyewitness
    identifications did not adequately protect a defendant’s right to due process under the
    Alaska Constitution.8 In its place, the court adopted a new test that was modeled after
    the test set forth by the New Jersey Supreme Court in State v. Henderson.9
    The supreme court had two principal reasons for adopting this new test.
    First, the Young test corrected some of the deficiencies of the former test by requiring
    trial judges to consider scientifically grounded variables when assessing the reliability
    of an out-of-court identification procedure conducted by the police.10 Second, in cases
    where the trial court does not order suppression of an out-of-court identification, the
    6
    Young, 374 P.3d at 426-28.
    7
    Brigman, 
    2020 WL 704854
    , at *1.
    8
    Young, 374 P.3d at 412.
    9
    Id. at 417-27 (citing State v. Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
     (N.J. 2011)).
    10
    Id. at 412-29; see Henderson, 27 A.3d at 918-22 (outlining the problems inherent in
    the prior eyewitness identification test under Braithwaite and explaining the variables to be
    considered under the new test).
    –5–                                          2724
    Young test prescribed improved jury instructions to assist jurors in evaluating the out-of­
    court identification according to these scientifically grounded variables.11
    Under Young, a trial court is required to consider two sets of variables when
    evaluating whether to suppress the results of an out-of-court eyewitness identification.
    The supreme court referred to the first group of variables as “system variables” (those
    variables that are within the control of the law enforcement officers who are conducting
    the identification procedure) and “estimator” variables (those variables intrinsic to the
    event that are not within the control of law enforcement).12
    Under the Young test, a defendant who seeks suppression of an out-of-court
    identification must show “some evidence,” tied to a system variable, tending to show that
    the identification procedure was tainted by suggestiveness that could lead to a mistaken
    identification.13 System variables include both how the identification was structured
    (e.g., whether it was the product of a showup, a lineup, or a photographic array) and the
    procedures surrounding the identification (e.g., the composition of the lineup, whether
    the witness received properly neutral pre-identification instructions or, instead, improper
    confirmatory feedback). Young outlines the following non-exhaustive list of system
    11
    Young, 374 P.3d at 428; see Henderson, 27 A.3d at 924-26 (directing the development
    of enhanced jury instructions).
    12
    Young, 374 P.3d at 416-25 (discussing variables).
    13
    Id. at 427. The supreme court explained that a suppression motion must generally be
    based on a system variable because state action is required to trigger a due process
    suppression analysis. Id.; see also Nichols v. Eckert, 
    504 P.2d 1359
    , 1362 (Alaska 1973)
    (“For [the due process] clause to apply there must be state action and the deprivation of an
    individual interest of sufficient importance to warrant constitutional protection.”). But see
    Henderson, 27 A.3d at 920 (recognizing this same requirement, but pointing to one New
    Jersey case, State v. Chen, 
    208 N.J. 307
     (2011), where conduct by a private actor was
    sufficient to trigger an evidentiary hearing).
    –6–                                          2724
    variables for a trial court to consider when determining whether a defendant has met this
    initial burden of showing “some evidence” of suggestiveness:
    1. Blind Administration. Was the identification procedure performed
    double-blind (i.e., was the person administering the procedure kept ignorant of the
    identity of the person who was suspected)? If double-blind administration of a photo
    lineup or physical lineup was impractical, did the police at least use a technique to ensure
    that the administrator had no knowledge of where the suspect appeared in the photo array
    or lineup?14
    2. Pre-identification Instructions. Did the administrator provide neutral
    pre-identification instructions to the witness being asked to make the identification,
    warning the witness that the suspect may not be present in the lineup and that the witness
    should not feel compelled to make an identification?15
    3. Lineup Construction. Did the photo array or lineup contain only one
    suspect embedded among at least five innocent fillers? Did the suspect stand out from
    other members of the lineup?16
    14
    Young, 374 P.3d at 417-18; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 897
     (“The consequences are
    clear: a non-blind lineup procedure can affect the reliability of a lineup because even the
    best-intentioned, non-blind administrator can act in a way that inadvertently sways an
    eyewitness trying to identify a suspect. An ideal lineup administrator, therefore, is someone
    who is not investigating the particular case and does not know who the suspect is.”).
    15
    Young, 374 P.3d at 418-19; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 897
     (“Pre-lineup instructions
    help reduce the relative judgment phenomenon . . . . Without an appropriate warning,
    witnesses may misidentify innocent suspects who look more like the perpetrator than other
    lineup members. . . . In one experiment, 45% more people chose innocent fillers in
    target-absent lineups when administrators failed to warn that the suspect may not be there.”).
    16
    Young, 374 P.3d at 419-20; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 897
     (“Properly constructed
    (continued...)
    –7–                                         2724
    4. Feedback and Recording Confidence. Did the witness receive any
    feedback from the police or from another witness before, during, or after the
    identification procedure? If the witness made a statement regarding their level of
    confidence in the identification, did the administrator record the witness’s statement of
    confidence immediately after the identification, before any possibility of confirmatory
    feedback?17
    5. Showups. Was the witness identified in a showup? Did the police
    perform the showup more than two hours after the event? Did the police warn the
    witness that the person they were about to view may not be the perpetrator, and that the
    witness should not feel compelled to make an identification?18
    6. Multiple Viewings. Was the witness exposed to the suspect after the
    crime but before making the identification? Did the witness fail to identify the suspect
    in an earlier procedure?19
    16
    (...continued)
    lineups test a witness’[s] memory and decrease the chance that a witness is simply
    guessing.”).
    17
    Young, 374 P.3d at 420; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 900
     (“Confirmatory feedback can
    distort memory. As a result, to the extent confidence may be relevant in certain
    circumstances, it must be recorded in the witness’[s] own words before any possible
    feedback. To avoid possible distortion, law enforcement officers should make a full record
    — written or otherwise — of the witness’[s] statement of confidence once an identification
    is made. Even then, feedback about the individual selected must be avoided.”).
    18
    Young, 374 P.3d at 420-21; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 903
     (“By their nature, showups
    are suggestive and cannot be performed blind or double-blind.”).
    19
    Young, 374 P.3d at 421; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 900
     (“Viewing a suspect more
    than once during an investigation can affect the reliability of the later identification. The
    problem . . . is that successive views of the same person can make it difficult to know
    (continued...)
    –8–                                         2724
    Again, Young states that a defendant need only show “some evidence” of
    suggestiveness from the above non-exhaustive list of system variables in order to obtain
    an evidentiary hearing on their suppression claim.20
    If the defendant establishes the need for an evidentiary hearing, the burden
    of production shifts to the State. The State must offer evidence that, even though the
    eyewitness identification procedure was potentially subject to suggestive influences
    arising from system variables, the identification is nevertheless reliable.21 However, the
    ultimate burden of persuasion remains with the defendant.22 In order to obtain
    suppression of the out-of-court identification (and any subsequent in-court identification
    by the witness), the defendant must show a “very substantial likelihood of irreparable
    misidentification.”23
    19
    (...continued)
    whether the later identification stems from a memory of the original event or a memory of
    the earlier identification procedure.”).
    20
    Young, 374 P.3d at 427.
    21
    Id. The State can also offer proof to rebut the defendant’s claim of suggestiveness.
    For example, if the defendant claims that there is “some evidence” that the witness received
    confirmatory feedback, the State may respond by introducing evidence to the contrary. If the
    court determines that the confirmatory feedback did not occur and that no other evidence of
    suggestiveness from a system variable otherwise exists, then suppression can be denied
    without additional analysis of the estimator variables. See Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 920
     (“[At
    the hearing, t]he State must then offer proof to show that the proffered eyewitness
    identification is reliable — accounting for system and estimator variables — subject to the
    following: the court can end the hearing at any time if it finds from the testimony that
    defendant’s threshold allegation of suggestiveness is groundless.”).
    22
    Young, 374 P.3d at 427.
    23
    Id. (quoting Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 920
    ).
    –9–                                         2724
    To evaluate whether the defendant has made the required showing, a trial
    court must consider both the system variables and the estimator variables described in
    Young.24 Again, estimator variables are those factors intrinsic to the event and not within
    the control of law enforcement, such as environmental variables and the characteristics
    of the witness and the suspect.25 Some examples of estimator variables the trial court
    must consider include:
    1. Stress. Did the witness view the perpetrator under particularly stressful
    conditions?26
    2. Weapons Focus. Was a weapon, or another unusual or distracting
    object, visible during the time the witness was viewing the perpetrator?27
    3. Duration of View.       How long was the witness able to see the
    perpetrator?28
    24
    
    Id.
    25
    
    Id. at 417
    .
    26
    
    Id. at 422
     (“Acknowledging the negative effect of stress on the reliability of
    eyewitness identifications may help jurors counteract the common misconception that faces
    seen in highly stressful situations can be burned into a witness’s memory.” (internal quotation
    marks and citation omitted)).
    27
    
    Id. at 422-23
    ; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 904-05
     (“When a visible weapon is used
    during a crime, it can distract a witness and draw his or her attention away from the culprit.”).
    28
    Young, 374 P.3d at 423.
    – 10 –                                          2724
    4. Environmental Conditions of View. What environmental conditions,
    such as distance and lighting, may have affected the witness’s ability to view the
    perpetrator?29
    5. Witness Characteristics. Were there any characteristics of the witness,
    such as mental and physical health, age, vision, or alcohol or drug use, that may have
    compromised the witness’s ability to see and identify the perpetrator?30
    6. Perpetrator Characteristics. Was the perpetrator disguised or otherwise
    difficult to describe? Has the suspect’s appearance changed since the crime?31
    7. Race and Ethnicity Bias.         Does the case involve a cross-racial
    identification?32
    8. Memory Decay/Retention Interval. How much time passed between the
    crime and the identification procedure?33
    9. Co-Witnesses. Did the witness discuss the identification or receive
    information about the suspect from co-witnesses or other non-state actors?34
    29
    Id.
    30
    Id. at 423-24.
    31
    Id. at 424 (“Masks, sunglasses, hats, hoods, and other things that hide the hair and
    hairline affect witnesses’ ability to accurately identify a perpetrator.”).
    32
    Id.
    33
    Id. at 424-25.
    34
    Id. at 425.
    – 11 –                                      2724
    This is not a complete list of the estimator variables that a trial court should
    consider.35    The Young test recognizes that the scientific research pertaining to
    eyewitness identification continues to evolve and that trial courts should take into
    account this developing research when assessing the reliability of eyewitness
    identifications.36
    One question that the Young opinion did not directly answer is whether the
    Biggers factors — the factors that were used in the former Anderson/Braithwaite test —
    are still valid factors to consider under the new test.
    Under Anderson/Braithwaite, a defendant was required to show that the
    procedures used to obtain the out-of-court eyewitness identification were “unnecessarily
    suggestive.”37 Once this showing was made, the trial court was required to consider “the
    totality of the circumstances” in determining whether the identification was nevertheless
    reliable enough to be admitted at trial.38 In assessing the totality of the circumstances,
    the court was required to consider the five factors articulated in Neil v. Biggers.39 These
    factors were: (1) the witness’s opportunity to view the suspect at the time of the crime;
    (2) the witness’s degree of attention; (3) the accuracy of any prior description of the
    suspect; (4) the level of certainty demonstrated by the witness during the identification
    35
    Id. at 427.
    36
    Id. (“Because of this, trial courts should not hesitate to take expert testimony that
    explains, supplements, or challenges the application of these variables to different fact
    situations.”).
    37
    Anderson v. State, 
    123 P.3d 1110
    , 1116 (Alaska App. 2005); Manson v. Braithwaite,
    
    432 U.S. 98
    , 114 (1977).
    38
    Anderson, 
    123 P.3d at 1116
    .
    39
    Braithwaite, 
    432 U.S. at
    114 (citing Neil v. Biggers, 
    409 U.S. 188
    , 199-200 (1972)).
    – 12 –                                        2724
    procedure; and (5) the time interval between the crime and the out-of-court
    identification.40
    Although the Young opinion did not directly address the current status of
    the Biggers factors, the supreme court indicated that its new test was modeled after the
    test adopted by the New Jersey Supreme Court in Henderson.41 And in Henderson, the
    New Jersey Supreme Court made clear that the Biggers factors should still be considered
    among the estimator variables, albeit with some important caveats.42 The New Jersey
    Supreme Court cautioned trial courts to be aware of the scientific criticism that the
    Biggers factors have generated. Chief among these criticisms is Biggers’s over-emphasis
    on a witness’s level of certainty.43
    Scientific studies have shown that a witness’s level of certainty is a poor
    indicator of the reliability of the witness’s identification — in part, because a witness’s
    level of certainty is often a product of suggestive procedures such as confirmatory
    feedback.44 Therefore, the New Jersey Supreme Court directed trial courts to consider
    whether the witness expressed high confidence in the identification “before receiving any
    feedback or other [confirmatory] information.”45 The New Jersey Supreme Court also
    encouraged trial courts to consider using protective orders to block the government from
    introducing evidence of a witness’s high degree of certainty, even when the witness’s
    40
    Biggers, 
    409 U.S. at 199-200
    .
    41
    Young, 374 P.3d at 427.
    42
    State v. Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
    , 918-22 (N.J. 2011).
    43
    Id. at 918.
    44
    Id.
    45
    Id. at 922 (emphasis added).
    – 13 –                                      2724
    out-of-court identification is not suppressed, if the witness’s level of certainty appears
    to have been influenced by suggestive procedures.46
    Another criticism of the Biggers framework, discussed in both Henderson
    and Young, is that three of the Biggers factors — the witness’s opportunity to view the
    crime, the witness’s degree of attention, and their level of certainty — often rely solely
    on the witness’s self-reporting.47      Studies have shown that witnesses frequently
    overestimate how long they saw the perpetrator and their degree of attention to the
    perpetrator.48 They also often underestimate the distance between themselves and the
    perpetrator.49 Therefore, to the extent possible, a trial court should use objective
    measures when assessing these estimator variables rather than rely exclusively on a
    witness’s self-reporting.
    Ultimately, the Young test requires a trial court to decide whether the
    defendant has shown, by a preponderance of the evidence, that the out-of-court
    identification procedure is tainted by a “very substantial likelihood of irreparable
    misidentification.”50 The trial court’s analysis of this question must be based on a
    46
    Id. at 925; cf. State v. Lawson, 
    291 P.3d 673
    , 695 (Or. 2012) (suggesting that trial
    courts can satisfy Evidence Rule 403 by admitting an eyewitness’s out-of-court identification
    but excluding the accompanying statement regarding the witness’s level of certainty, because
    such statements can often be persuasive to jurors but actually have limited reliability).
    47
    Young v. State, 
    374 P.3d 395
    , 425-26 (Alaska 2016); Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 918
    .
    48
    Young, 374 P.3d at 420 n.140, 423.
    49
    Id. at 423.
    50
    Id. at 427 (quoting Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 920
    ).
    – 14 –                                        2724
    consideration of both the system variables and the estimator variables.51 The court’s
    analysis should also be informed by accepted scientific studies regarding the inherent
    perils that can lead to flawed eyewitness identifications.52
    If the trial court finds that the defendant has shown a “very substantial
    likelihood of irreparable misidentification,” the trial court must suppress the out-of-court
    identification and preclude the witness from providing any in-court identification.53 If
    the trial court finds that the defendant has not made the requisite showing, the court must
    still provide the kind of tailored jury instructions described in Young to ensure that jurors
    understand the factors that can affect the reliability of an out-of-court eyewitness
    identification.54
    The Young test applied to the facts of Brigman’s case
    Here, there is no question that Brigman met his initial burden of showing
    “some evidence” of suggestiveness related to a system variable, because the out-of-court
    identification in Brigman’s case was obtained through a showup — an inherently
    suggestive procedure.
    In a showup, a single suspect is presented to a witness, and the witness is
    asked whether they can make an identification.               As courts have frequently
    51
    
    Id.
    52
    
    Id.
    53
    
    Id.
    54
    
    Id. at 428
    ; see Henderson, 
    27 A.3d at 920
     (“If the evidence is admitted, the court
    should provide appropriate, tailored jury instructions . . . .”).
    – 15 –                                        2724
    acknowledged, showups are “inherently suggestive.”55 By their very nature, they cannot
    be performed using a blind or double-blind format.
    Moreover, in contrast to lineups and photographic arrays, where a witness
    with a faulty memory might well pick someone other than the suspect, every positive
    identification in a showup implicates the suspect.56 As the Alaska Supreme Court noted
    in Young, “Showups seemingly provide little protection against witnesses who are
    inclined to guess, as witnesses participating in showups tend to base their identifications
    on clothing.”57 Research also shows that “an innocent suspect who resembles the actual
    perpetrator is more likely to be incorrectly identified in a showup than in a lineup.”58
    Despite these problems, showups still play a role in the criminal justice
    system because they are often the best way to determine the direction of an investigation
    under exigent circumstances. For example, in Brigman’s case, the police needed to learn
    whether they had apprehended the intoxicated driver who had just fled the crash site or
    whether they should continue their search. Because showups are useful and often
    necessary, the out-of-court identifications resulting from showups remain potentially
    admissible under the Young test.
    But under the Young test, the fact that a showup may have been necessary
    under the circumstances does not end the analysis. Instead, a trial court is required to
    assess all of the relevant system and estimator variables, with particular attention to how
    55
    Anderson v. State, 
    123 P.3d 1110
    , 1117 (Alaska App. 2005).
    56
    Young, 374 P.3d at 421.
    57
    Id.
    58
    Id.
    – 16 –                                      2724
    the showup was conducted and when it occurred.59 Studies have shown that showups
    are often most reliable if they are conducted immediately after the crime when the
    witness’s memory is fresh.60       But research also shows that the likelihood of a
    misidentification at a showup increases significantly in as little as two hours after the
    event.61
    In addition, studies have shown that the pre-identification instructions the
    witness receives are an important influence on the reliability of the witness’s
    identification.62 To increase the chances of reliability, a witness should be instructed that
    the person they are about to view may or may not be the culprit and that they should not
    feel compelled to make any identification.63
    In Brigman’s case, the superior court found that the showup occurred
    within minutes of the time when the witness, Thomas Jenkins, encountered and
    conversed with the driver of the crashed vehicle. The record supports this finding, and
    the parties appear to agree that less than thirty minutes passed from the time of the crash
    to the time of the showup.
    The superior court also found that Officer Childers provided the appropriate
    pre-identification instructions to Jenkins. Prior to the identification, Childers instructed
    Jenkins that the person in custody might or might not be the driver. These instructions
    were recorded and are not in dispute.
    59
    Id. at 427.
    60
    Id. at 421.
    61
    Id.
    62
    Id. at 418-19.
    63
    Id.
    – 17 –                                        2724
    Based on the immediacy of the showup and the fact that Jenkins received
    neutral pre-identification instructions, the superior court found that the showup
    identification that occurred in Brigman’s case was “the type the Supreme Court had in
    mind when it said [that showups] can be reliable.”
    But, as the superior court recognized, finding that the showup was
    conducted in an appropriately neutral form is not the end of the analysis under Young.
    The superior court still needed to assess the estimator variables to determine whether,
    notwithstanding the immediacy of the showup and the result-neutral pre-identification
    instructions, there was a “very substantial likelihood of irreparable misidentification.”64
    The superior court’s order provides an in-depth analysis of the estimator
    variables in Brigman’s case. The court found, for example, that “there is no suggestion
    that the witness’s stress level was particularly high[,] because he was not in any danger
    and his desire was to help the driver of the vehicle that had just crashed in front of him.”
    The court also found that, although the interaction between Jenkins and the driver was
    short, their encounter was face-to-face and in an area that was well-lit by a streetlight.
    There were no weapons or unusual or distracting objects present, and the driver was not
    wearing a hat or other disguise. The court noted that there was no issue of cross-racial
    identification bias, as both Jenkins and the driver were white. In addition, Jenkins, a
    sixty-one-year-old man, was wearing his eyeglasses and was not under the influence of
    alcohol or medication. The court further found that Jenkins had acted responsibly by
    first checking on the driver of the vehicle and then calling 911 and providing a relatively
    detailed description. The court noted that there were some discrepancies regarding
    Jenkins’s description of the driver’s clothing and hair. But the court found that these
    64
    See id. at 427.
    – 18 –                                       2724
    discrepancies were explainable, that they did not undermine the reliability of the
    identification, and that these discrepancies could be understood and considered by the
    jury.
    In sum, after conducting a thorough analysis under Young of how and when
    the showup occurred, and after analyzing the applicable estimator variables, the court
    concluded that Brigman had failed to show that there was a very substantial likelihood
    of irreparable misidentification, and thus the showup identification was sufficiently
    reliable to be introduced into evidence.
    On appeal, Brigman focuses on the discrepancies between Jenkins’s
    description of the driver and what Brigman looked like at the showup. According to
    Brigman, these discrepancies were significant enough that the superior court should have
    found the identification unreliable.
    We disagree. In his initial 911 call, Jenkins described the driver as a man,
    likely white, with black hair, gray clothing, and a zipped hoodie. Based on this
    description, Officer Fraize stopped Brigman, a white man with black hair and gray/dark
    clothing. It is true that Brigman was not wearing a hoodie, and we agree with Brigman
    that the absence of a hoodie casts some doubt on the reliability of Jenkins’s
    identification, and that this was a proper subject for cross-examination at trial. But this
    one discrepancy does not defeat the superior court’s conclusion that Brigman failed to
    show a very substantial likelihood of irreparable misidentification, given the other
    system and estimator variables at play in this case.
    Brigman also asserts that Jenkins misidentified Brigman’s hair. According
    to Brigman, Jenkins described the driver as having hair that was longer, curlier, and
    wavier than Brigman’s. But the transcript page that Brigman cites does not support this
    – 19 –                                     2724
    claim. Instead, it appears that it was the other eyewitness who originally described the
    driver as having “shoulder-length” wavy hair.
    The record does, however, show that Jenkins himself acknowledged that
    his recollection of the driver’s hair differed somewhat from the way Brigman’s hair
    looked at the time of the showup. After Jenkins identified Brigman as the driver of the
    crashed vehicle, Jenkins noted that the driver’s hair “wasn’t messed up [like Brigman’s]
    at the time.” But according to the record, Brigman was sweaty and nervous by the time
    of the showup and, as the superior court noted, there was a light rain or mist “which
    could have altered the look of a person’s hair.” We therefore find no error in the superior
    court’s discounting of this supposed discrepancy.
    Ultimately, under Young, it is the defendant who bears the burden of
    proving that the system variables and estimator variables applicable to the case give rise
    to a very substantial likelihood of misidentification.65 We find no error in the superior
    court’s conclusion that Brigman did not meet this burden.
    Conclusion
    The judgment of the superior court is AFFIRMED.
    65
    Id.
    – 20 –                                      2724
    

Document Info

Docket Number: A12727

Filed Date: 4/22/2022

Precedential Status: Precedential

Modified Date: 4/22/2022