State of Iowa v. Earl Booth-Harris ( 2020 )


Menu:
  •                    IN THE SUPREME COURT OF IOWA
    No. 18–0002
    Filed April 24, 2020
    STATE OF IOWA,
    Appellee,
    vs.
    EARL BOOTH-HARRIS,
    Appellant.
    On review from the Iowa Court of Appeals.
    Appeal from the Iowa District Court for Des Moines County, John G.
    Linn, Judge.
    A defendant seeks further review of a court of appeals decision
    rejecting his due process challenge to a police photo array identification
    procedure and the court’s failure to grant relief on his claim that trial
    counsel was ineffective for failing to request different jury instructions on
    eyewitness identifications reflecting scientific research.   DECISION OF
    COURT OF APPEALS AFFIRMED IN PART AND VACATED IN PART;
    DISTRICT COURT JUDGMENT AFFIRMED.
    Mark C. Smith, State Appellate Defender (until withdrawal), and Nan
    Jennisch, Assistant Appellate Defender, for appellant.
    Thomas J. Miller, Attorney General, and Louis S. Sloven, Assistant
    Attorney General, for appellee.
    2
    WATERMAN, Justice.
    In this appeal, a defendant convicted of first-degree murder based
    in part on eyewitness testimony seeks a new trial on two grounds. First,
    he contends the police used unduly suggestive photographic identification
    procedures and the district court erred by failing to grant his motion to
    suppress the resulting identification.     Second, he contends his trial
    counsel was ineffective for failing to request more robust jury instructions
    on eyewitness identifications that reflect modern scientific research. The
    district court, without objection, had given the Iowa State Bar Association
    (ISBA)   Uniform    Criminal   Jury   Instruction   200.45   on   eyewitness
    identification.   We transferred the case to the court of appeals, which
    affirmed his conviction while preserving his ineffective-assistance-of-
    counsel claims for possible postconviction proceedings. We granted the
    defendant’s application for further review.
    On our review, we decline the defendant’s invitation to change our
    constitutional precedent to further limit the admissibility of eyewitness
    identifications following police photo arrays.      We determine that the
    double-blind procedures used in this case, with an appropriate admonition
    given the witness, were not unduly suggestive. Unlike the court of appeals,
    we determine the record is adequate to decide the ineffective-assistance-
    of-counsel claim challenging ISBA Instruction 200.45, and we reject the
    claim on the merits. For the reasons explained below, we affirm the district
    court’s judgment of conviction.
    I. Background Facts and Proceedings.
    The trial testimony established the following facts. On February 16,
    2015, in Burlington, Iowa, an argument broke out between Deonte Carter
    and Terrance Polk in the front yard of Rita Lewis’s home. Carter accused
    Polk of breaking into his home and stealing a pair of sneakers. Lewis told
    3
    the men to take their fight elsewhere, and the disputants dispersed. Polk
    and Carter then communicated through Facebook and set a time and place
    to fight.
    That afternoon, Carter, along with his cousin, Donnell Watson, and
    friend, Edward DeWitt, arrived at the park on 7th and Elm in Burlington.
    Polk showed up with several men who had accompanied him earlier at the
    Lewis house. All of the men were the same race. Carter and his group
    were approached by a shooter wearing a black stocking cap whom Watson
    later identified as Earl Booth-Harris. The shooter and Carter engaged in
    a brief exchange of words with Carter telling the shooter, “[D]o what you
    gotta do.” The shooter opened fire, hitting Carter multiple times. Watson
    ran away when the shooting started. When it stopped, he returned and
    found Carter on the ground bleeding from bullet wounds. DeWitt called
    911. Watson found a .40 caliber gun on the ground next to Carter and
    took it to Lewis’s house. Police recovered that weapon later. Carter died
    due to gunshot wounds to his chest, abdomen, and back. Carter was shot
    by a .45 caliber gun.
    The same day, Booth-Harris presented to a hospital in Monmouth,
    Illinois, for a gunshot wound to his leg. Booth-Harris was shot with a .40
    caliber gun. In an interview with the police at the hospital, Booth-Harris
    stated that he was in the area of the shooting and saw an argument
    involving several men. He told police he heard gunshots and ran and while
    running away was shot. Booth-Harris told the police that he went home,
    changed clothes, and contacted his father, who took him to the Illinois
    hospital. Booth-Harris feared going to the hospital in Burlington where he
    might be shot. Booth-Harris denied participating in the shooting.
    On the day of the shooting, Watson gave a statement to police and
    was presented with a photo array. A photo of Polk was included because
    4
    police suspected he was the shooter. This array did not include a photo
    of Booth-Harris. Watson did not identify anyone in these photos as the
    shooter. Watson was next presented with a single photo of Booth-Harris.1
    He denied knowing who Booth-Harris was.
    Two days later, Watson was again interviewed and shown photo
    arrays prepared by Detective Josh Tripp.                Detective Tripp “pick[ed]
    photographs of subjects that look[ed] similar to the suspect that [they]
    ha[d] at the time.” Detective Tripp personally picked six photographs out
    of ten to twelve that he believed looked the most similar. Sergeant Chad
    McCune, who was not involved in the investigation and did not know who
    was a suspect, presented the photo array in a double-blind protocol.
    Sergeant McCune read to Watson a photographic admonition, which
    Watson signed before looking at the photos. The admonition states,
    You are about to view a photographic line-up. The person who
    committed the crime may or may not be included in it. While
    looking at the photographs, keep an open mind that the
    individuals may not appear exactly as they did on the date of
    the crime. Their hairstyles, facial hair, clothing, etc. may have
    changed. Also, photographs may not always depict the true
    complexion of a person, who may be lighter or darker than
    shown in the photo. The officer showing you the photographs
    has no knowledge of the incident. In the line-up process, the
    photographs will be shown to you one at a time and are not in
    any specific order. Take as much time as you need to look at
    each photograph. Even if you identify an individual, the
    officer will continue to show you all of the photographs. The
    officer is not allowed to tell you whether your choice, if you
    make one, is a suspect in the investigation. Do not tell other
    witnesses that you have or have not identified anyone.
    1When   presented with the picture of Booth-Harris, Officer Derek Schwandt
    testified,
    Q. Why did you show him the picture then? A. Well, we just had
    a shooting in Burlington and there’s a subject with a gunshot wound. We
    don’t know if he’s a victim. We don’t know if he’s a suspect. We don’t
    know if he’s a bystander, so at that time, we’re not sure what his
    involvement was.
    5
    A photo of Booth-Harris was included in this second array, and Watson
    identified him as likely being the shooter after quickly dismissing the other
    five photographs. Watson commented that he wanted to say Booth-Harris
    was the shooter, but he stated his eyes were smaller in the photo than
    they were the day of the shooting; however, Watson noted favorably the
    “strong jaw structure” of Booth-Harris and indicated that was the “only
    thing he could kind of see.” Watson also called attention to Booth-Harris’s
    eyebrows, stating that the eyebrows of the shooter were thicker. At this
    point, Watson said he had a fifty percent certainty and initialed the
    picture. Watson told the officer, “[Y]’all can like take another picture and
    show me.”
    The officers informed Watson that they were going to try to find more
    recent photos.   While Watson was still at the station, Detective Tripp
    prepared another array. Sergeant McCune again administered the array
    and again read the admonition to Watson, which he signed. Watson again
    quickly dismissed the other photographs, and he identified Booth-Harris
    as the shooter. When asked about his level of certainty, Watson this time
    said he was seventy percent certain. Watson commented about “feelin’
    like” it was him.    After an exchange between Watson and Sergeant
    McCune, in which Sergeant McCune stated that “feelin’ like it” means more
    than seventy percent, Watson ended by stating he had one hundred
    percent certainty.
    A search of Booth-Harris’s home yielded evidence used at trial.
    Blood drops outside led into the home. A black stocking cap and bloody
    t-shirt were found inside the home. A .45 caliber shell casing was located
    on the ground outside the back door. The casing matched the .45 caliber
    casings at the scene of the shooting, indicating they were fired from the
    same gun. Additionally, live .45 caliber rounds were recovered from Booth-
    6
    Harris’s home. An expert identified the live rounds as the same brand of
    casings as those at the shooting and opined the casings at the scene of the
    shooting and those at the Booth-Harris house were likely manufactured
    around the same time.
    Booth-Harris was charged with murder in the first degree, in
    violation of Iowa Code section 707.2 (2015), a class “A” felony. Booth-
    Harris filed a motion to suppress the identification, arguing the procedure
    was impermissibly suggestive and a violation of his due process rights.
    The district court denied the motion to suppress. Watson testified at trial
    and identified Booth-Harris as the shooter. Booth-Harris was found guilty
    of first-degree murder and was sentenced to life in prison without the
    possibility of parole. Booth-Harris appealed, raising two issues. First,
    Booth-Harris claimed the district court erred in denying his motion to
    suppress Watson’s in-court identification because the out-of-court
    identification was impermissibly suggestive and unreliable.                 Second,
    Booth-Harris claimed his trial counsel was ineffective for failing to request
    jury instructions similar to those adopted in State v. Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
    (N.J. 2011). 2
    We transferred the case to the court of appeals, which affirmed
    Booth-Harris’s conviction while preserving for possible postconviction
    relief action his “due process claim raised under the Iowa Constitution and
    his claim defense counsel should have requested a different eyewitness
    identification instruction.”      We granted Booth-Harris’s application for
    further review.
    2Booth-Harris’s trial counsel offered no expert testimony on the reliability of
    eyewitness identifications, and his appellate counsel makes no claim Booth-Harris
    received constitutionally deficient representation based on the lack of such expert
    testimony.
    7
    II. Standard of Review.
    We review constitutional challenges to eyewitness testimony
    de novo. State v. Taft, 
    506 N.W.2d 757
    , 762 (Iowa 1993). As we recently
    reiterated,
    “When a defendant challenges a district court’s denial of a
    motion to suppress based upon the deprivation of a state or
    federal constitutional right, our standard of review is de novo.”
    We examine the whole record and “make ‘an independent
    evaluation of the totality of the circumstances.’ ” “Each case
    must be evaluated in light of its unique circumstances.”
    State v. Fogg, 
    936 N.W.2d 664
    , 667 (Iowa 2019) (quoting State v. Coffman,
    
    914 N.W.2d 240
    , 244 (Iowa 2018)).
    Claims of ineffective assistance of counsel are reviewed de novo.
    State v. Harrison, 
    914 N.W.2d 178
    , 188 (Iowa 2018).
    III. Analysis.
    A. The Motion to Suppress.                 We begin with Booth-Harris’s
    argument that the district court erred in denying his motion to suppress
    Watson’s pretrial identification of him as Carter’s killer.              Booth-Harris
    contends the pretrial photographic identification procedures violated his
    due process rights under the Federal and Iowa Constitutions. 3 See U.S.
    Const. amend. XIV; Iowa Const. art. I, § 9. Booth-Harris has the burden
    of proving that the identification procedures were unconstitutionally
    suggestive or unreliable. State v. Neal, 
    353 N.W.2d 83
    , 86 (Iowa 1984).
    We apply a long-standing, two-part analysis to challenges to out-of-
    court identifications, the same test set by the United States Supreme Court
    and utilized by most other states. 
    Taft, 506 N.W.2d at 762
    ; J.P. Christian
    3The court of appeals determined that Booth-Harris failed to preserve error on his
    due process claim under the Iowa Constitution because he did not argue for a different
    standard in his motion to suppress. Booth-Harris argues that error was preserved in his
    motion to suppress because he alleged due process claims under the Federal and Iowa
    Constitutions, and the district court ruled on them. We agree with Booth-Harris that
    error was preserved.
    8
    Milde, Bare Necessity: Simplifying the Standard for Admitting Showup
    Identifications, 60 B.C. L. Rev. 1771, 1806, 1823 (2019) (stating “[t]he
    majority of state high courts apply the federal standard that the Supreme
    Court reiterated in [Manson]” and collecting cases that “follow the [Manson]
    test and apply the [Neal v.] Biggers[, 
    409 U.S. 188
    , 
    93 S. Ct. 375
    (1972),]
    factors with little or no divergence”); Lawrence Rosenthal, Eyewitness
    Identification and the Problematics of Blackstonian Reform of the Criminal
    Law, 110 J. Crim. L. & Criminology 181, 205–06 & nn.132–133 (2020)
    [hereinafter Rosenthal, Eyewitness Identification] (identifying forty-one
    states and the District of Columbia that utilize the test articulated in
    Manson v. Brathwaite, 
    432 U.S. 98
    , 
    97 S. Ct. 2243
    (1977), and noting
    “[m]ost courts, when invited to depart from Manson as a matter of state
    law, have declined to do so”). “First, we decide whether the procedure used
    for the identification was impermissibly suggestive.” 
    Taft, 506 N.W.2d at 762
    . If we determine the procedure was impermissibly suggestive, we turn
    to the second step to decide whether “under the totality of [the]
    circumstances the suggestive procedure gave rise to a very substantial
    likelihood of irreparable misidentification.”
    Id. (alteration in
    original)
    (quoting State v. Whetstine, 
    315 N.W.2d 758
    , 764 (Iowa 1982)).
    Under the second step, the critical question is whether the out-of-
    court identification was reliable.
    Id. We have
    endorsed the prevailing five-
    factor test for assessing reliability of out-of-court identification procedures
    adopted from Biggers:
    (1) the opportunity of the witness to view the perpetrator at
    the time of the crime, (2) the witness’ degree of attention,
    (3) the accuracy of the witness’ prior description of the
    perpetrator, (4) the level of certainty demonstrated by the
    witness at the confrontation, and (5) the length of time
    between the crime and the confrontation.
    9
    Id. at 763;
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 114
    , 97 S. Ct. at 2253; 
    Biggers, 409 U.S. at 199
    –200, 93 S. Ct. at 382. “When unnecessarily suggestive pretrial out-
    of-court identification procedures conducive to mistaken identification
    that are incapable of repair are used, the Due Process Clause requires
    exclusion of the testimony of the identification.” State v. Folkerts, 
    703 N.W.2d 761
    , 763 (Iowa 2005). There is no Due Process Clause violation
    as long as the identification has sufficient aspects of reliability. State v.
    Webb, 
    516 N.W.2d 824
    , 830 (Iowa 1994). If Booth-Harris cannot satisfy
    our two-part test, “the identification evidence and its shortcomings or
    credibility are for the jury to weigh.” 
    Neal, 353 N.W.2d at 87
    .
    Booth-Harris urges us to modify our approach to eyewitness
    identification evidence given empirical research showing the accuracy and
    trustworthiness of eyewitness identifications are limited by estimator
    variables and system variables. 4 Booth-Harris asserts that the current
    test does not account for these variables and that some of the Biggers
    factors are at odds with the research. Booth-Harris asks us to incorporate
    these system and estimator variables when determining whether an
    identification is unduly suggestive and unreliable and to abandon the
    second step of our test. We decline to alter our test under the Iowa or
    Federal Due Process Clauses. The two-part test remains our law.
    We acknowledge the evolving social science research without
    concluding that it serves as “a basis for establishing fixed principles of
    4“Estimator   variables” are the factors related to reliability that are “connected to
    the event, witness, or perpetrator—items over which the justice system has no control.”
    State v. Lujan, 
    459 P.3d 992
    , 1001 (Utah 2020). Examples of estimator variables are the
    conditions such as lighting or distance that may have affected the witness’s view, any
    stress or duress that the witness incurred, weapon focus, witness characteristics,
    perpetrator characteristics, and memory deterioration factors.
    Id. “System variables”
    are
    defined as “factors controlled by the court or law enforcement” such as the use of double-
    blind identification procedures, detailed instructions for the witness before conducting
    the identification procedure, and a proper lineup.
    Id. 10 constitutional
    law.” See State v. 
    Lujan, 459 P.3d at 992
    , 999 (Utah 2020).
    This research has not persuaded the United States Supreme Court or the
    overwhelming majority of other state supreme courts to alter or abandon
    the two-part test.   Our adherence to stare decisis is supported by the
    recent decisions of the Utah and Wisconsin Supreme Courts that retreated
    from their earlier decisions relying on the scientific research to alter the
    test for admissibility of eyewitness identifications under state due process
    provisions.
    In Lujan, the Utah Supreme Court clarified an earlier opinion that
    had expanded on the Biggers factors for the admissibility of eyewitness
    identification testimony under the due process clause of the Utah
    Constitution.
    Id. at 997–99
    (discussing State v. Ramirez, 
    817 P.2d 774
    (Utah 1991)). The unanimous Lujan court stated,
    The Ramirez opinion looked only to evolving social science in
    its articulation of the reliability factors that it identified. It
    based the factors on “well-respected and essentially
    unchallenged empirical studies” as laid out in State v. Long,
    
    721 P.2d 483
    (Utah 1986), even while conceding that the
    holding in Long “was not squarely based on the state
    constitution.” The opinion established this “more empirically
    based approach” solely because the court “judge[d] this to be
    a more appropriate approach.”
    These sorts of considerations—rooted in evolving social
    science and legal scholarship—may be appropriate grounds
    for our provision of “guidance” on the reliability of eyewitness
    identification testimony. But such evolving grounds are not a
    basis for establishing fixed principles of constitutional law.
    And our decision in Ramirez nowhere offered an originalist
    basis for constitutionalizing the reliability factors set forth in
    that opinion.
    Id. at 999
    (alteration in original) (citations omitted) (quoting 
    Ramirez, 817 P.2d at 780
    ). Lujan held the Ramirez factors could provide guidance under
    the rules of evidence but could no longer serve as a constitutionally
    11
    required test for admissibility under the state due process clause.
    Id. at 999
    –1000. We agree.
    We join the Lujan court in recognizing the development in the
    research and the use of estimator and system variables, but we echo that
    “research in this field is ongoing.”
    Id. at 1001.
    As such, we agree with the
    Utah Supreme Court that other methods such as the evidentiary
    “rulemaking process lends itself nicely to adaptation over time in response
    to developments in scientific and legal scholarship in this important field.”
    Id. at 995.
    “[A]s our understanding of the factors that affect the reliability
    of eyewitness testimony develops, our application and understanding of
    our rules of evidence can likewise evolve.”
    Id. at 1001.
    Similarly, in State v. Roberson, the Wisconsin Supreme Court
    rejected the defendant’s argument that a victim’s identification of the
    defendant should be suppressed because it began with law enforcement
    showing the victim a single Facebook photograph of the defendant. 
    935 N.W.2d 813
    , 815–16 (Wis. 2019). The Wisconsin Supreme Court expressly
    overruled its prior decision that departed from the United States Supreme
    Court     decisions   in   Manson   and      Biggers     and    “was    based   on
    misunderstanding       [those   decisions]     in      regard   to     out-of-court
    identifications and on topical social science.”
    Id. at 822
    . 
    The Roberson
    court aptly held that given the tendency of scholars to embody the
    subjective beliefs of the time period, “social science research cannot be
    used to define the meaning of a constitutional provision.”
    Id. at 820.
    We
    agree.
    This is not to suggest that social science does not play a role in
    challenging the admissibility of eyewitness testimony under Iowa Rule of
    Evidence 5.403 or through expert testimony or modified jury instructions.
    The ISBA Jury Instruction Committee is welcome to evaluate revisions to
    12
    Iowa Uniform Jury Instruction 200.45. We anticipate that a new task force
    will evaluate revisions to the Iowa Rules of Evidence.              Meanwhile, as
    always, defense counsel can cross-examine witnesses and argue the
    weight to be given their testimony. Counsel may also consider introducing
    expert testimony regarding the social science research. But we decline to
    defer to social science to raise our constitutional bar to admissibility. We
    trust Iowa juries to give the testimony appropriate weight.
    1. The photographic identification procedures were not impermissibly
    suggestive. Turning to the first step in our analysis, we must determine
    whether the photographic identification procedures were impermissibly
    suggestive. “It must be conceded that even the most well-designed and
    well-applied pretrial identification procedure will be, to some extent,
    suggestive.” State v. Walton, 
    424 N.W.2d 444
    , 447 (Iowa 1988).
    The officers showed Watson a photo of Booth-Harris on three
    occasions.    First, on the day of the shooting after Watson viewed the
    photographic array that included Polk’s photograph and made no
    identification, 5 officers showed Watson a photo of Booth-Harris in a single-
    photographic display. Watson did not identify Booth-Harris at that time.
    Second, two days later, Watson was shown a photographic array that
    included Booth-Harris’s photo when he stated he was fifty percent sure
    that Booth-Harris was the shooter but could not say so definitively given
    the way his eyes looked. Lastly, that same day, the officers showed Watson
    another photographic array that included a more recent photo of Booth-
    Harris, and Watson identified him as the shooter.
    Booth-Harris      contends     the     single-photographic     display    was
    impermissibly suggestive by making him stand out and appear familiar to
    5Booth-Harris’sphoto was not shown to Watson in the first photographic array on
    February 16, 2015, and he did not identify any of those individuals as the shooter.
    13
    Watson when Booth-Harris appeared again in the subsequent two
    photographic arrays. He claims it was unnecessary for the officers to show
    Watson his photograph in a single array given that there were no exigent
    circumstances and a photographic array could have been prepared and
    presented to Watson instead.      Booth-Harris relies heavily on State v.
    Lawson, 
    291 P.3d 673
    (Or. 2012) (en banc).
    In Lawson, the Oregon Supreme Court outlined the existing
    research and eight system variables, or the best practices surrounding the
    eyewitness identification procedures.
    Id. at 686–87.
    First, the research
    recommends that the identification procedures be conducted by a “blind”
    administrator who does not know the identity of the suspect and therefore
    cannot purposely or unintentionally suggest that information to the
    witness.
    Id. at 686.
         Secondly, it recommends preidentification
    instructions that tell the witness that a suspect may or may not be in the
    lineup or photo array and that it is acceptable to not make an
    identification.
    Id. It notes
    that such an instruction significantly decreases
    the likelihood of misidentification.
    Id. Third, “lineup
    fillers should be
    selected first on the basis of their physical similarity with the witness’s
    description of the perpetrator” and then based on their similarity to the
    suspect if there is no description.
    Id. Fourth, the
    research favors a sequential showing of the photographs
    one at a time over a simultaneous viewing of the photos as a group because
    witnesses are more likely to make an absolute judgment rather than a
    relative judgment.
    Id. Fifth, the
    research states that showups, or
    procedures when the officer presents the witness with a single suspect for
    identification, are generally less reliable because the witness then knows
    who the police believe is a suspect.
    Id. However, the
    Lawson court noted
    that
    14
    [w]hen conducted properly and within a limited time period
    immediately following an incident, a showup can be as reliable
    as a lineup. A showup is most likely to be reliable when it
    occurs immediately after the witness has observed a criminal
    perpetrator in action because the benefit of a fresh memory
    outweighs the inherent suggestiveness of the procedure.
    Id. Sixth, the
    research warns that “[v]iewing a suspect multiple times
    throughout the course of an investigation can adversely affect the
    reliability of any identification that follows those viewings.”
    Id. Seventh, Lawson
    identifies    concerns with suggestive wording and leading
    questions.
    Id. at 687.
    And lastly, eighth, research warns that “[p]ost-
    identification confirming feedback tends to falsely inflate witnesses’
    confidence in the accuracy of their identifications, as well as their
    recollections concerning the quality of their opportunity to view a
    perpetrator and an event.”
    Id. Most of
    Lawson’s identified best practices for conducting eyewitness
    identification procedures were followed by the officers here.     Sergeant
    McCune, who administered the second and third photograph arrays, was
    not involved in the criminal investigation and did not know who the
    suspect was. In each of those subsequent photographic arrays, Sergeant
    McCune read the photographic identification admonition form to Watson,
    and he signed it. The form reflects the best practice since it instructs the
    witness that the person who committed the crime may or may not be
    included in the photographic array, to “keep an open mind that the
    individuals may not appear exactly as they did on the date of the crime,”
    and to take as much time as necessary to look at each photograph. The
    people used as “lineup fillers” were chosen by another detective because
    they looked the most similar to the suspect.         For each array, the
    photographs were shown sequentially, one at a time. Watson quickly ruled
    out the five other photographs (in the first array), but paused on
    15
    photograph four, which was Booth-Harris. In the second array, Watson
    identified Booth-Harris again and stated the eyes were a closer match to
    the shooter’s.
    Although the officers showed Watson a single photograph of Booth-
    Harris, at that time, they did not consider him to be a suspect, and they
    did not know if he had a connection to the crime.            Booth-Harris’s
    photograph was not presented to Watson as a potential shooter; it was
    merely used to ask Watson if he knew who Booth-Harris was.              This
    occurred on the same day as the crime. This single-photographic array is
    not sufficient to taint the identification procedure as impermissibly
    suggestive.
    Neal is instructive. 
    353 N.W.2d 83
    (Iowa 1984). In Neal, the victim
    had been abducted and sexually abused one evening but was able to
    escape her captor.
    Id. at 85.
    Shortly after the assault, and while the victim
    was in the hospital, the police showed her a set of photographs that did
    not include the defendant’s picture.
    Id. at 87.
    She made no identification.
    Id. Six days
    later, the police showed her five mugshots, and “[a]lthough
    she would not make a positive identification, she did point out [the
    defendant] as most closely resembling her assailant.”
    Id. Approximately two
    weeks later, a second array of four photos was prepared that contained
    a more recent photo of the defendant.
    Id. at 89.
    This time, the victim
    positively identified the defendant as her assailant.
    Id. The defendant
    in Neal asserted that the second photo array was
    tainted by the fact that the victim had already been exposed to him
    through the image from the first photo array.
    Id. He claimed
    it was
    possible that she was identifying him based on the image from the first
    photo array rather than from the image of the person who assaulted her.
    Id. We held
    that the “defendant’s first picture did not mislead the victim
    16
    into making the subsequent identification” because she did not make an
    identification during the first photo array and there were distinct
    differences between the images such that the second photo, which more
    closely resembled how the defendant looked around the time of the
    assault, portrayed a different hairstyle and a more mature individual.
    Id. As in
    Neal, we do not find that the first photo misled Watson into
    making the subsequent identification. Watson did not initially identify
    Booth-Harris in the single photograph array, and he was careful not to
    select an individual whose facial features did not match his memory of the
    shooter.   The photograph in the second array additionally showed a
    different angle and portion of Booth-Harris’s face to reflect the portion of
    the shooter’s face that Watson saw.      Watson took care not to identify
    anyone until the facial features matched his memory of the shooter. See
    State v. Rawlings, 
    402 N.W.2d 406
    , 408 (Iowa 1987) (holding that the
    identification was not impermissibly suggestive despite the fact that the
    defendant was the only individual whose picture was repeated in the two
    arrays because “[a] reasonable effort to harmonize the lineup is normally
    all that is required”). Further, at trial, Watson stated that he had lied to
    the police about not knowing Booth-Harris when he was shown the single
    photograph array.
    Q. Well, isn’t it a fact that you saw [Officer] Derek
    Schwandt at the police station on February 16th? A. I guess
    so.
    Q. Did you tell the truth to [Officer] Derek Schwandt
    about what happened that afternoon? A. No.
    Q. What did you not tell him the truth about? A. The
    picture lineup.
    Q. I’m sorry? A. The picture lineup.
    Q. What do you mean, the picture lineup? A. Like,
    when they was showing me the pictures and stuff like that, I
    lied about the person who it was.
    17
    Q. What lie did you tell? A. That I didn’t know who it
    was.
    The credibility of Watson’s identifications was for the jury to decide.
    Additionally, Booth-Harris contends the photographic identification
    procedures were impermissibly suggestive due to Watson’s inflation of his
    level of certainty that Booth-Harris was the killer from seventy percent to
    one hundred percent after Sergeant McCune’s prompting.                    Even
    “[a]ssuming, without deciding, that the photographic identification
    procedure employed by the police here was ‘suggestive,’ it does not
    necessarily follow that the procedure was ‘impermissibly’ suggestive.”
    State v. Mark, 
    286 N.W.2d 396
    , 404 (Iowa 1979). We discourage officers
    from urging the witness to increase their level of certainty. But we decline
    to find that Sergeant McCune’s comments after Watson had identified
    Booth-Harris require exclusion of the identification. Again, the jury could
    evaluate     Watson’s   credibility   in   light   of   Sergeant   McCune’s
    postidentification comments.
    The person that created the photo arrays, Detective Tripp, was not
    the same individual who showed the arrays to Watson, Sergeant McCune.
    In fact, Sergeant McCune was not involved in the investigation; did not
    know which of the photographs depicted the suspect; and, therefore, could
    not have signaled whether or not Watson correctly identified the suspect.
    As stated, Sergeant McCune’s lack of involvement with, or knowledge of,
    the case was purposeful and in line with the best practices. Since Sergeant
    McCune did not know whether or not Booth-Harris was the suspect, his
    potential encouragement that Watson increase his level of confidence in
    his identification was not impermissibly suggestive. The availability of
    video evidence of their interaction further ensured the jury could make its
    18
    own determination on the reliability of Watson’s level of certainty in his
    identification.
    For the above reasons, we do not find that Watson’s identification of
    Booth-Harris as the shooter was impermissibly suggestive.
    2. The photographic identification procedures were reliable.        To
    assess reliability under the second factor of our analysis, we turn to the
    five-factor Biggers test.
    (1) the opportunity of the witness to view the perpetrator at
    the time of the crime, (2) the witness’ degree of attention,
    (3) the accuracy of the witness’ prior description of the
    perpetrator, (4) the level of certainty demonstrated by the
    witness at the confrontation, and (5) the length of time
    between the crime and the confrontation.
    
    Taft, 506 N.W.2d at 763
    .     Veering from the Biggers test, Booth-Harris
    contends that the identification was unreliable for a variety of reasons:
    Watson did not initially identify him in the single-photographic display, it
    was a high-stress situation, Watson could not see Booth-Harris’s face or
    did not get a good look at him, weapon focus can affect reliability, Watson’s
    certainty does not amount to reliability, there was a time delay between
    the incident and the identification, and Watson’s drug use negatively
    impacted the accuracy of the identification.
    As stated above, we reject Booth-Harris’s invitation to abandon the
    Biggers factors. We will review each factor in turn. Watson had ample
    opportunity to view the shooter. Watson’s attention was focused on the
    individual who he saw with a gun before he ran away when shots were
    fired. Watson acknowledged that his view of the shooter’s face was from
    his nose to his forehead, and he particularly focused on the shooter’s eyes.
    Watson’s description of the shooter was largely accurate with the exception
    of his height estimate. Watson identified Booth-Harris as the shooter in
    two of the three arrays, and he indicated his level of certainty in the
    19
    identification each time. Additionally, Watson identified Booth-Harris’s
    photograph and pointed out how the facial features matched that of the
    shooter whereas he quickly dismissed the other photographs in the array.
    Lastly, only two days passed between the incident and the positive
    identification. See 
    Mark, 286 N.W.2d at 406
    (holding that a timespan of
    one week between the incident and the identification was insufficient to
    defeat the reliability of the identification). Altogether, under the totality of
    the circumstances, the five factors weigh in favor of reliability.
    We acknowledge that “[t]he reliability of eyewitness identification
    can be affected by a number of variables.” State v. Doolin, ___ N.W.2d ___,
    ___ (Iowa 2020) (filed today).    The additional factors that Booth-Harris
    argues should be considered when assessing reliability all go to the weight
    of Watson’s identification, not admissibility. The fact that it was a high-
    stress situation and that Watson was under the influence of drugs is
    insufficient to exclude his identification. “Most evidence can be called into
    question in some way; however, that does not give the . . . court the ability
    to preclude admission. We have cross-examination for a reason; evidence
    often is tested in that way.” 
    Roberson, 935 N.W.2d at 828
    . The jury is
    responsible for weighing the evidence. As such,
    [w]e are content to rely upon the good sense and judgment of
    [our] juries, for evidence with some element of
    untrustworthiness is customary grist for the jury mill. Juries
    are not so susceptible that they cannot measure intelligently
    the weight of identification testimony that has some
    questionable feature.
    
    Mark, 286 N.W.2d at 405
    (quoting 
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 116
    , 97 S. Ct. at
    2254). “The jury may be an imperfect vehicle for assessing eyewitness
    evidence, but it is the vehicle for resolving guilt or innocence found in the
    Constitution. We can have little confidence that a judge-made substitute
    20
    will do better.” Rosenthal, Eyewitness Identification, 110 J. Crim. L. &
    Criminology at 243.
    The Roberson court correctly noted that “not all showings of a single
    photo are infected by improper police influence causing a very substantial
    likelihood of misidentification.     Each identification must be evaluated
    based on its own 
    facts.” 935 N.W.2d at 826
    . After applying the Biggers
    factors, the Roberson court determined that the single photographic array
    did not result in a substantial likelihood of misidentification and “the jury
    should decide whether [the defendant] was correctly identified.”
    Id. at 827–28.
    Similarly, we do not believe that there is a substantial likelihood
    of irreparable misidentification under the totality of the circumstances
    here.
    We conclude the photo array identification was not impermissibly
    suggestive and unreliable.      Therefore, the district court did not err in
    denying Booth-Harris’s motion to suppress Watson’s photo array
    identification of him.
    B. Uniform Jury Instruction.
    We now turn to Booth-Harris’s ineffective-assistance-of-counsel
    claim.      The district court submitted Iowa’s uniform instruction on
    eyewitness identification to the jury.      See Iowa State Bar Ass’n, Iowa
    Criminal Jury Instruction 200.45. Booth-Harris asserts on appeal that
    his trial counsel breached his duty by failing to request a more thorough
    eyewitness identification instruction that incorporated system and
    estimator variables and that he was prejudiced as a result. We disagree,
    and we determine that Booth-Harris’s trial counsel did not provide
    constitutionally deficient representation by failing to request a different
    jury instruction.
    21
    To prevail on an ineffective-assistance-of-counsel claim, Booth-
    Harris must prove that his trial counsel (1) failed to perform an essential
    duty and (2) prejudice resulted. State v. Clay, 
    824 N.W.2d 488
    , 495 (Iowa
    2012) (describing the two-prong test for ineffective-assistance-of-counsel
    claims set out in Strickland v. Washington, 
    466 U.S. 668
    , 687, 
    104 S. Ct. 2052
    , 2064 (1984)). We presume counsel performed competently unless
    the claimant proves otherwise by a preponderance of the evidence.
    Id. Counsel’s performance
    is measured objectively against the prevailing
    professional norms after considering all the circumstances.
    Id. “Trial counsel
    has no duty to raise an issue that lacks merit . . . .”
    State v. Ortiz, 
    905 N.W.2d 174
    , 184 (Iowa 2017); see also State v. Graves,
    
    668 N.W.2d 860
    , 881 (Iowa 2003) (same).          Counsel has a duty to be
    “familiar with the current state of the law.” State v. Hopkins, 
    576 N.W.2d 374
    , 379–80 (Iowa 1998). But “[w]e do not expect counsel to anticipate
    changes in the law, and counsel will not be found ineffective for a lack of
    ‘clairvoyance.’ ” Millam v. State, 
    745 N.W.2d 719
    , 722 (Iowa 2008). As
    such, “[i]n situations where the merit of a particular issue is not clear from
    Iowa law, the test ‘is whether a normally competent attorney would have
    concluded that the question . . . was not worth raising.’ ”
    Id. (alteration in
    original) (quoting 
    Graves, 668 N.W.2d at 881
    ).         The record must be
    adequate to enable us to resolve an ineffective-assistance-of-counsel claim
    on direct appeal. State v. Ary, 
    877 N.W.2d 686
    , 704 (Iowa 2016).
    To establish the second prong of the test, prejudice, “the claimant
    must prove by a reasonable probability that, but for counsel’s failure to
    perform an essential duty, the result of the proceeding would have been
    different.”
    Id. at 705.
    This does not require a showing that counsel’s
    conduct “more likely than not altered the outcome in the case,” but rather
    that “the probability of a different result is ‘sufficient to undermine [our]
    22
    confidence in the outcome’ of the trial.”
    Id. (alteration in
    original) (quoting
    
    Graves, 668 N.W.2d at 882
    ).
    We begin our analysis with the text of ISBA Criminal Jury
    Instruction 200.45, which states,
    The reliability of eyewitness identification has been raised as
    an issue. Identification testimony is an expression of belief or
    impression by the witness.        Its value depends on the
    opportunity the witness had to see the person at the time of
    the crime and to make a reliable identification later.
    In evaluating the identification testimony of a witness,
    you should consider the following:
    1. If the witness had an adequate opportunity to see
    the person at the time of the crime. You may consider such
    matters as the length of time the witness had to observe the
    person, the conditions at that time in terms of visibility and
    distance, and whether the witness had known or seen the
    person in the past.
    2. If an identification was made after the crime, you
    shall consider whether it was the result of the witness’s own
    recollection.   You may consider the way in which the
    defendant was presented to the witness for identification, and
    the length of time that passed between the crime and the
    witness’s next opportunity to see the defendant.
    3. Any identification made by picking the defendant out
    of a group of similar individuals is generally more reliable than
    one which results from the presentation of the defendant
    alone to the witness.
    4. Any occasion in which the witness failed to identify
    the defendant or made an inconsistent identification.
    Iowa State Bar Ass’n, Iowa Criminal Jury Instruction 200.45 (2015). The
    instruction’s comment notes that it is “provided for use when appropriate
    under State v. Tobin, 
    338 N.W.2d 879
    (Iowa 1983).”
    Booth-Harris contends that this model instruction is insufficient
    and that his counsel should have requested an alternative instruction or
    a modified model instruction that better informs the jury about system
    and estimator variables and educates them on memory recollection. He
    cites to other jurisdictions that have criticized the use of the federal
    23
    framework and instead encouraged and ultimately incorporated such
    enhanced instructions.        See Commonwealth v. Gomes, 
    22 N.E.3d 897
    (Mass. 2015); Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
    ; 6 N.J. Criminal Model Jury
    Instructions, Identification: In-Court and Out-of-Court Identifications
    (2012), https://www.njcourts.gov/attorneys/criminalcharges.html.
    However, despite the research relied upon by the Massachusetts and
    New Jersey courts, there is a growing body of academic literature that
    questions the efficacy of certain provisions in such jury instructions on
    eyewitness identifications. In fact, recent studies have shown that the
    more comprehensive jury instructions like New Jersey’s Henderson
    instruction can actually overcorrect the problem. See Abigail Twenter,
    Striking     the   Right   Balance:   Mitigating   the   Effects    of   Eyewitness
    Misidentification in Missouri, 75 J. Mo. B. 14, 16 (2019) (noting the studies
    that support this theory). Results of a study that tested the efficacy of the
    instruction in a simulated murder trial with 335 mock jurors “indicated
    [that] the [Henderson] jury instruction was more likely to indiscriminately
    increase the rate of exonerations for all defendants, not just those who are
    innocent.”
    Id. “Ideally, an
    instruction should help jurors discriminate
    good eyewitness testimony from bad,” but studies that have tested the
    effect of the Henderson instruction have shown it does not accomplish this
    goal.    Elizabeth F. Loftus, Eyewitness Science and the Legal System,
    14 Ann. Rev. L. & Soc. Sci. 1, 6 (2018).
    As one scholar noted, these studies, such as the one that found that
    6A commentator recently observed that “Henderson’s protections may prove
    illusory.” Rosenthal, Eyewitness Identification, 110 J. Crim. L. & Criminology at 201,
    222–23. On remand, the Henderson trial court “conducted a hearing, made findings
    regarding each of the relevant systems and estimator variables, and then denied the
    motion to suppress [the witness’s] identification.”
    Id. at 201.
    The New Jersey
    intermediate appellate court upheld that ruling, and the New Jersey Supreme Court
    denied Henderson’s petition seeking further review.
    Id. 24 the
    reduction in the conviction rate occurred for both the
    strong and the weak case . . . suggest that scholars need to
    keep working to find new ways to improve the jury
    instructions so that they do not merely induce general
    skepticism but also improve sensitivity.
    Id.; see also Nicholas A. Kahn-Fogel, The Promises and Pitfalls of State
    Eyewitness Identification Reforms, 
    104 Ky. L
    .J. 99, 119 (2015–2016) (“Yet
    there are reasons to believe that even the most detailed instructions might
    be insufficient to cause jurors to incorporate fully the results of scientific
    research into their decision-making. . . .        [T]his research has not
    demonstrated superior outcomes with detailed instructions, and leading
    scientists have concluded that it remains an ‘open question’ whether
    detailed instructions can have a significant impact on juries.”); Rosenthal,
    Eyewitness Identification, 110 J. Crim. L. & Criminology at 218 (“[S]tudies
    of the jury instructions utilized in New Jersey since the Henderson decision
    indicate that the new instructions cause mock jurors to become more
    skeptical of all eyewitness identifications, regardless of the strength of the
    evidence.”); John T. Wixted & Gary L. Wells, The Relationship Between
    Eyewitness Confidence and Identification Accuracy: A New Synthesis, 18
    Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. 10, 11–13 (2017) (casting doubt on part of
    New Jersey’s Henderson instruction and similar instructions adopted by
    Massachusetts, Connecticut, and other states that suggest eyewitness
    confidence is generally an unreliable indicator of accuracy because
    research   indicates   those   jury   instructions   fail   to   “appropriately
    communicate the high information value of an initial statement of
    confidence obtained from a pristine identification procedure”). Another
    scholar recently surveyed research studies, concluding the Henderson jury
    instructions are less effective than expected.
    Nor did we observe a significant impact on the verdict
    and non-verdict measures in interaction with case strength,
    indicating that Henderson instructions did not produce
    25
    sensitivity to eyewitness identification evidence.        These
    findings are in contrast to the Henderson Court’s assumption
    that the new researched-based case-specific instructions
    would help jurors to objectively evaluate eyewitness evidence.
    In fact, results indicate that Henderson instruction might not
    only fail to induce sensitivity but also might induce skepticism
    among jurors instead. These results are consistent with
    previous research findings ([Athan P.] Papailiou et al., [The
    Novel New Jersey Eyewitness Instruction Induces Skepticism
    but Not Sensitivity, PLoS ONE 10(12) (Dec. 2015)];
    [Marlee Kind] Dillon et al., [Henderson Instructions: Do They
    Enhance Evidence Evaluation?, 17(1) J. of Forensic Psych.
    Res. & Prac. 1 (2017)]; [Angela M.] Jones et al., [Comparing the
    Effectiveness of Henderson Instructions and Expert Testimony:
    Which Safeguard Improves Jurors’ Evaluations of Eyewitness
    Evidence?, 13 J. of Experimental Criminology 29 (2017)];
    [Angela M.] Jones & [Steven D.] Penrod, [Improving the
    Effectiveness of the Henderson Instruction Safeguard Against
    Unreliable Eyewitness Identification, 24(2) Psych., Crime & L.
    177 (2017)]). While the former two studies found that
    Henderson instructions led to an overall skepticism, the two
    latter studies found lack of any effect on the verdict. These
    studies also examined the effectiveness of Henderson
    instructions     on    different    samples     (undergraduates,
    community members), using different media (video,
    transcript), and types of crime (from robbery to murder) and
    different timing of the instructions (before or after eyewitness
    testimony), always with null findings. Altogether, the results
    suggest that Henderson instructions are not as effective as
    they were meant to be.
    Radim Koníček, Assessment of Eyewitness Testimony Accuracy: Effect of
    Different Type of Instructions on Delivering Guilty or Not Guilty Verdicts 68
    (April 27, 2018) (unpublished Master’s thesis, Masaryk University) (on file
    with author). Another scholar more broadly observed:
    [T]he data are noisy, sometimes inconsistent, and provide
    nothing approaching a clear indication that reforms that
    reduce the risk of suggestion are likely to have a meaningful
    effect on the rate of false identifications—much less benefits
    that exceed their costs. The data are chaotic, and the state of
    our knowledge about eyewitness identification reform remains
    primitive.
    Rosenthal, Eyewitness Identification, 110 J. Crim. L. & Criminology at 216.
    Given the evolving research and debate in this area, trial counsel did
    not provide constitutionally deficient representation by failing to request
    26
    an alternative instruction or additions to the uniform instruction. When
    the researchers themselves are uncertain about the best practice for jury
    instructions, and when some recent research rejects utilizing the very type
    of instructions that Booth-Harris now desires, we cannot expect counsel
    to predict which side will prevail. Counsel did not breach his duty by
    failing to object to the ISBA Instruction 200.45, an instruction we have
    never held misstates the law.
    We reiterate that “we are slow to disapprove of the uniform jury
    instructions.” State v. Ambrose, 
    861 N.W.2d 550
    , 559 (Iowa 2015); see
    also State v. Beets, 
    528 N.W.2d 521
    , 523 (Iowa 1995) (per curiam) (same);
    State v. Monk, 
    514 N.W.2d 448
    , 450 (Iowa 1994) (en banc) (same); State v.
    McMullin, 
    421 N.W.2d 517
    , 518 (Iowa 1988) (“[W]e normally approve the
    submission of uniform instructions . . . .”); State v. Weaver, 
    405 N.W.2d 852
    , 855 (Iowa 1987) (same); State v. Jeffries, 
    313 N.W.2d 508
    , 509 (Iowa
    1981) (same); State v. Whiteside, 
    272 N.W.2d 468
    , 471 (Iowa 1978) (same);
    Ness v. H.M. Iltis Lumber Co., 
    256 Iowa 588
    , 594, 
    128 N.W.2d 237
    , 240
    (1964) (same); McMaster v. Hutchins, 
    255 Iowa 39
    , 45, 
    120 N.W.2d 509
    ,
    512 (1963) (same). The uniform instructions are valuable to the bench
    and bar.
    “While we normally approve the submission of uniform
    instructions,” we are free to find a “particular instruction is
    faulty.” Everyone knows this. What some readers may fail to
    fully appreciate, however, is the tremendous service the
    members of the ISBA Jury Instruction Committee do for our
    justice system. Without the uniform instructions, trial judges
    and lawyers statewide would be burdened reinventing the
    wheel researching and drafting ad hoc jury instructions every
    trial. The variances in the wording of instructions would
    increase exponentially, further burdening appellate review. It
    is far better to have a committee of dedicated trial judges and
    lawyers craft uniform instructions to spare their colleagues
    that time and trouble. If an appellate court concludes a
    particular jury instruction is erroneous, or if our court
    changes the law in a manner requiring a revision, then
    corrections to the uniform instruction can be readily made
    27
    and implemented statewide. The value of our current process
    is well understood by the bench and bar.
    
    Ambrose, 861 N.W.2d at 563
    (Waterman, J., concurring) (quoting
    
    McMullin, 421 N.W.2d at 518
    ).
    Of course, the ISBA Jury Instructions Committee is welcome to
    revisit Instruction 200.45 and recommend changes to the instruction for
    the ISBA Board of Governors to adopt or decline.             “[T]he court is not
    required to give any particular form of an instruction; rather, the court
    must merely give instructions that fairly state the law as applied to the
    facts of the case.”   State v. Marin, 
    788 N.W.2d 833
    , 838 (Iowa 2010),
    overruled on other grounds by Alcala v. Marriott Int’l, Inc., 
    880 N.W.2d 699
    ,
    708 & n.3 (Iowa 2016). We hold that the ISBA Criminal Jury Instruction
    200.45 does not misstate the law.
    We hold that Booth-Harris’s trial counsel had no duty to object to
    Instruction 200.45 or to request a different instruction on eyewitness
    identifications.   Because Booth-Harris failed to prove the first element
    required to prevail on an ineffective-assistance-of-counsel claim, a breach
    of duty, we end our analysis there. See 
    Graves, 668 N.W.2d at 869
    (“A
    defendant’s inability to prove either element is fatal.”).
    IV. Disposition.
    For the foregoing reasons, we affirm the court of appeals decision in
    part and vacate in part.      We affirm the district court’s judgment of
    conviction and denial of the motion to suppress.
    DECISION OF COURT OF APPEALS AFFIRMED IN PART AND
    VACATED IN PART; DISTRICT COURT JUDGMENT AFFIRMED.
    All justices concur except Appel, J., who dissents.
    28
    #18–0002, State v. Booth-Harris
    APPEL, Justice (dissenting).
    The comfortable conventional canard is this: “Juries are not so
    susceptible that they cannot measure intelligently the weight of
    identification testimony that has some questionable feature.” Manson v.
    Brathwaite, 
    432 U.S. 98
    , 116, 
    97 S. Ct. 2243
    , 2254 (1977). From the
    beginning, this proposition was doubtful, but we now know after decades
    of scientific evidence that the opposite is true. Studies have shown that
    our wonderfully honest jurors often hold intuitive views about the
    accuracy of eyewitness testimony that are simply incorrect. Cindy Laub &
    Brian H. Bornstein, Juries and Eyewitnesses, in Encyclopedia of
    Psychology and Law 390, 390–92 (Brian L. Cutler ed., 2008); see Jules
    Epstein, Irreparable Misidentifications and Reliability: Reassessing the
    Threshold for Admissibility of Eyewitness Identification, 58 Vill. L. Rev. 69,
    90 (2013) [hereinafter Epstein, Irreparable Misidentification] (citing a 2011
    study surveying over 1800 people in the United States that showed that
    63% agreed the memory works like a video camera where we can review
    and inspect the event later and 47.6% agreed that once experiencing an
    event and a memory is formed it does not change). The jurors need our
    help.
    In fact, because it is one of the most powerful pieces of evidence that
    can be presented to a jury, eyewitness testimony is the leading cause of
    wrongful convictions. Richard A. Wise et al., How to Analyze the Accuracy
    of Eyewitness Testimony in a Criminal Case, 
    42 Conn. L
    . Rev. 435, 441
    (2009) [hereinafter Wise et al., Criminal Eyewitness Testimony]. With the
    significant role eyewitness testimony plays in our criminal justice system,
    we must adapt our legal system to our scientific understanding in order to
    29
    ensure that convictions are not being obtained based on eyewitness
    misidentifications.
    Our system of justice currently tolerates an unacceptably high risk
    of misidentification. The due process standard developed decades ago by
    the United States Supreme Court under the United States Constitution,
    and uncritically applied by this court under the Iowa Constitution,
    requires only a “modicum of potential reliability” for admission of
    eyewitness identification. Epstein, Irreparable Misidentification, 58 Vill. L.
    Rev. at 71. As a corollary to the very low standard of admissibility, we
    endorse the convenient and self-satisfying illusion that the adversarial
    system and cross-examination provide jurors with the kind of information
    needed to intelligently weigh the evidence in a fashion sufficient to afford
    a defendant a fair trial.
    But we now know better. For reasons beyond my comprehension,
    the eyewitness science is not harnessed by the majority. But our system
    today utilizes what amounts to stone-aged principles. The declaration that
    “this is how it has always been done” is inadequate. As the great Oliver
    Wendell Holmes observed,
    It is revolting to have no better reason for a rule of law than
    that so it was laid down in the time of Henry IV. It is still more
    revolting if the grounds upon which it was laid down have
    vanished long since and the rule simply persists from blind
    imitation of the past.
    Oliver Wendell Holmes, The Path of the Law, address dedicating a new hall
    at Boston University School of Law (January 8, 1897), in 10 Harv. L. Rev.
    457, 469 (1897), as quoted in Varnum v. Brien, 
    763 N.W.2d 862
    , 877 (Iowa
    2009).
    Further, our criminal justice system should not be a conveyor belt
    designed efficiently to produce convictions, affirmed on appeal, in
    30
    sufficient number to meet the perceived demands of law and order. A court
    system that singularly focuses on achieving convictions bends toward a
    brand of state authoritarianism that should not be acceptable in a
    democratic society.
    Instead, we must zealously pursue with grit and determination twin
    goals: that the guilty are convicted and the innocent go free. See Berger v.
    United States, 
    295 U.S. 78
    , 88, 
    55 S. Ct. 629
    , 633 (1935). In order to
    achieve these twin goals, it is imperative that we fashion our legal rules to
    conform to our developing scientific knowledge on the critically important
    question of eyewitness identification. That is what I have tried to do in
    Doolin and in this opinion.
    If we applied a legal structure that is consistent with and based upon
    what we know about eyewitness identification, the conviction in this case
    would be reversed. I therefore respectfully dissent.
    I. Introduction.
    There is no need here to repeat the extensive overview of the
    evolution of eyewitness testimony science given in State v. Doolin, ___
    N.W.2d ___ (Iowa 2020) (Appel, J., dissenting), which I incorporate by
    reference. There is, however, a body of science that relates directly to the
    deficiencies presented in the identification of Earl Booth-Harris that did
    not receive extended treatment in Doolin. First, I review that eyewitness
    science below.
    Second, I review the facts of the eyewitness identification in this
    case.    As will be seen, from the very beginning there were important
    estimator and system variables that significantly decreased the overall
    reliability of the identification later made by Donnell Watson. Further, the
    identification process in this case was unnecessarily suggestive in several
    respects. In short, the identification was highly unreliable.
    31
    Third, I examine the principles of due process under the United
    States and Iowa Constitutions. I discuss how the Iowa Constitution can
    provide greater protections for Iowans when facing possible eyewitness
    testimony.    Under the Iowa Constitution, we can incorporate widely
    accepted scientific knowledge and utilize those developments to create a
    system that advances the goal of a fundamentally fair criminal justice
    system even if the United States Supreme Court declines to do so under
    the United States Constitution. I conclude that fundamental fairness, in
    light of the science, requires a new due process approach to the admission
    of evidence of eyewitness identification.
    Finally, I discuss whether defense counsel was ineffective in not
    requesting a science-based eyewitness identification instruction to aid the
    jury in this case. The Iowa State Bar Association (ISBA) instruction on
    eyewitness identification, based on a case from the 1970s, has not been
    modified to reflect the over forty years of research in the areas of cognition,
    recall, and perception. The ISBA eyewitness identification jury instruction
    became obsolete about twenty years ago.
    The time for a change in the instruction has not simply arrived: it
    has long since passed. A competent criminal attorney who has followed
    the development of the law would know that and should have requested
    an appropriate modern eyewitness identification instruction that can be
    found in the caselaw and scholarly literature. I conclude that the failure
    to seek a science-based eyewitness identification instruction amounted to
    ineffective assistance of counsel.
    II. Variables & the Identification.
    A. Eyewitnesses, Juries, and the Science.
    1. Introduction. As noted in the Doolin dissent, many states, though
    not yet Iowa, have started to embrace the vast and extensive knowledge of
    32
    eyewitness science in their judicial system. See, e.g., Young v. State, 
    374 P.3d 395
    , 417–26 (Alaska 2016); State v. Guilbert, 
    49 A.3d 705
    , 720–25
    (Conn. 2012); Brodes v. State, 
    614 S.E.2d 766
    , 770–71 (Ga. 2005); State
    v. Cabagbag, 
    277 P.3d 1027
    , 1034–39 (Haw. 2012); Commonwealth v.
    Gomes, 
    22 N.E.3d 897
    , 907–17 (Mass. 2015); State v. Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
    , 896–913 (N.J. 2011); State v. Lawson, 
    291 P.3d 673
    , 685–88 (Or.
    2012) (en banc); State v. Long, 
    721 P.2d 483
    , 488–91 (Utah 1986). 7
    It is virtually undisputed that over the past four decades, “serious
    concerns have been raised about the potential unreliability of eyewitness
    identification evidence.”        Gary L. Wells et al., Policy and Procedure
    Recommendations for the Collection and Preservation of Eyewitness
    Identification Evidence, 44 Law & Hum. Behav. 3, 4 (2020) [hereinafter
    Wells et al., Policy & Procedures].               When it comes to eyewitness
    identifications, an “[a]ccurate eyewitness identification requires that a
    witness to a crime correctly sense, perceive, and remember objects and
    events that occurred and recall them later.”                   Nat’l Acad. of Scis., 8
    Identifying the Culprit: Assessing Eyewitness Identification 9 45 (2014)
    [hereinafter Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit]. Accordingly, the
    accuracy of the witness’s identification depends on the limits of the
    witness’s “sensation, perception, and memory.”
    Id. Current research
    provides greater and more thorough insight into how these systems can
    7For  more states and cases embracing scientific research when evaluating
    eyewitness testimony see Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at ___ n.1.
    8The National Academy of Sciences is a private, nonprofit society, established by
    an Act of Congress in 1863 charged with providing “independent, objective advice to the
    nation on matters related to science and technology” and “committed to furthering
    science in America.” Mission, Nat’l Acad. of Scis., http://www.nasonline.org/about-
    nas/mission/ [https://perma.cc/WK7W-K36D].
    9In  preparing the report, the committee heard from numerous experts,
    practitioners, and stakeholders and reviewed relevant, published and unpublished,
    works in the relevant scientific arena. Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 2.
    33
    misdirect, misperceive, and fail the witness. In addition to the science
    outlined in the Doolin dissent, there are several other points of scientific
    consensus that have a bearing in the case before us.
    2. Perception, memory, and noise.      Both vision and memory are
    contaminated by noise—factors that lead to uncertainty by the observer
    about whether a particular signal is present.
    Id. at 47.
    In vision, noise
    can be in the form of lighting, glares, shadows, obstructions, loud or
    distracting sounds, and other sources relevant and not relevant to the
    sensory content.
    Id. Specific to
    vision, when a person views an object, person, or event,
    a complex process in the form of light refraction, photoreceptors, and
    sensory processing occur.
    Id. at 50
    (outlining the process and research).
    While some factors are inherent to the visual system of the perceiver,
    others are dependent upon the viewing conditions, such as time of
    exposure and lighting. Both work together to influence the quality and
    accuracy of the information gained by the observer.
    Id. at 50
    –51. Even
    further, the National Academy of Sciences asserted that under the typical
    viewing conditions associated with a typical crime, “[the] source of noise
    may place severe limitations on the ability of the observer to sense key
    pieces of information that are not present at the center of gaze.”
    Id. at 51.
    Similar to vision, memory is susceptible to noise. Encoding memory,
    storing memory, and remembering does not occur in a vacuum, unaffected
    by the outside environment.
    Id. at 59–
    60. 
    Instead, the fidelity of our
    memories face compromise at various stages in the process. “Without
    awareness, we regularly encode events in a biased manner and
    subsequently forget, reconstruct, update, and distort the things we believe
    to be true.”
    Id. at 60.
    As such, the memory of an eyewitness is malleable
    and requires care in testing.          Brandon L. Garrett, Eyewitness
    34
    Identifications and Police Practices: A Virginia Case Study, 2 Va. J. Crim.
    L. 1, 3 (2014).
    3. Estimator variables. Factors independent of the criminal justice
    system are referred to estimator variables and empirical studies explain
    that a wide range of those variables may have a significant effect on
    eyewitness accuracy. Clifford S. Fishman & Anne T. McKenna, 7 Jones on
    Evidence § 61:10 (7th ed.), Westlaw (database updated July 2019).
    Estimator variables are for augmenting or discounting the credibility of
    witnesses. Gary L. Wells, Applied Eyewitness-Testimony Research: System
    Variables and Estimator Variables, 36 J. Personality & Soc. Psychol. 1546,
    1546 (1978). I turn to the estimator variables most pertinent to the Booth-
    Harris identification, focusing on those impacting vision and memory, and
    how they discount the credibility of the eyewitness.
    a. Perpetrator characteristics—disguise, familiarity, and own-race
    bias.    Disguises negatively impact identification accuracy.      Jamal K.
    Mansour et al., Impact of Disguise on Identification Decisions and
    Confidence with Simultaneous and Sequential Lineups, 36 Law & Hum.
    Behav. 513, 513–14 (2012) [hereinafter Mansour et al., Disguise and
    Identification] (discussing the various ways disguises can impact
    identification, such as imparting less identifying information, highlighting
    that disguises influence attention allocation by the witness and can
    decrease the amount of information able to be encoded). The Mansour
    study supported the postulation that the more disguised the target’s face
    was the less likely a study participant was to make an accurate lineup
    decision.
    Id. at 523.
    Additionally, the likelihood of erroneous identification
    depended not only on the degree of disguise but also on what parts of the
    face are disguised.
    Id. at 523–24.
    Disguises function as a type of noise
    35
    influencing a witness’s ability to fully take in and process information. See
    Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 69.
    Adding to the complexity of identifications, depending on the
    observer’s familiarity with the face, their ability to recognize the person
    varies. Angus F. Chapman et al., How Robust Is Familiar Face Recognition?
    A Repeat Detection Study of More Than 1000 Faces, Royal Soc’y Open Sci.,
    2,   10   (2018),    https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/
    rsos.170634; Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 68. In fact,
    people are “remarkably poor” at matching unfamiliar faces. Ahmed M.
    Megreya & A. Mike Burton, Unfamiliar Faces Are Not Faces: Evidence from
    a Matching Task, 34 Memory & Cognition 865, 865 (2006). In Wade, the
    Supreme Court cautioned that the identification of a stranger is
    “proverbially untrustworthy.” United States v. Wade, 
    388 U.S. 218
    , 228,
    
    87 S. Ct. 1926
    , 1933 (1967).
    Cross-race identification—identification of individuals outside of
    one’s own racial group—is consistently worse than own-race identification.
    Michael P. Seng & William K. Carroll, Eyewitness Testimony: Strategies
    and Tactics § 2:23 (2d ed.), Westlaw (database updated Nov. 2019);
    Christian A. Meissner & John C. Brigham, Thirty Years of Investigating the
    Own-Race Bias in Memory for Faces: A Meta-Analytic Review, 7 Psychol.,
    Pub. Pol’y, & L. 3, 3 (2001) (reviewing results of thirty years of research on
    own-race bias in the memory of faces); Andrew E. Taslitz, “Curing” Own
    Race Bias: What Cognitive Science and the Henderson Case Teach About
    Improving Jurors’ Ability to Identify Race-Tainted Eyewitness Error, 16
    N.Y.U. J. Legis. & Pub. Pol’y 1049, 1052 & n.18 (2013) (discussing the
    extensive research regarding “own race bias” and how the effect results in
    eyewitnesses of one race being more likely to misidentify innocent persons
    when the innocent person is of another race).
    36
    b. Duration of exposure. Longer exposure duration—time available
    to view the perpetrator—is generally associated with a witness’s ability to
    subsequently identify the perpetrator. Gary L. Wells et al., Eyewitness
    Evidence: Improving Its Probative Value, 7 Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. 45, 53–
    54 (2006) [hereinafter Wells et al., Eyewitness Evidence]. Legally, exposure
    duration has long been thought of as a factor to be considered when
    evaluating eyewitness testimony. 10                 Research confirms exposure’s
    significance, finding “relatively long exposure duration produces greater
    accuracy.” Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 97–98. Exposure
    may interact with, and affect, other variables as well.
    c. Stress and attention.          The face of the perpetrator following a
    traumatic event is often said to be “burned into someone’s memory” or in
    making an identification, someone may claim that they’ll “never forget” the
    face. Contrary to the popular belief that stress heightens one’s ability to
    perceive and memorize, research actually suggests the opposite. Elizabeth
    F. Loftus, Ten Years in the Life of an Expert Witness, 10 Law & Hum. Behav.
    241, 254–55 (1986) [hereinafter Loftus, Ten Years]. Instead, high stress
    or fear can affect eyewitness identification impacting both vision and
    memory. Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 94; Loftus, Ten
    Years, 10 Law & Hum. Behav. at 254–55 (explaining the Yerkes-Dodson
    Law as a theoretical relationship between stress and memory finding low
    stress and high stress impair attention); Wise et al., Criminal Eyewitness
    Testimony 
    42 Conn. L
    . Rev. at 456, 505–06; Wells et al., Eyewitness
    10See   
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 108
    , 97 S. Ct. at 2250 (considering eyewitness
    testimony that focused on the distance between the officer and seller, the duration of the
    interaction, and lighting, ultimately noting that the officer “certainly was paying attention
    to [the] identity [of] the seller”); Neil v. Biggers, 
    409 U.S. 188
    , 200, 
    93 S. Ct. 375
    , 382
    (1972) (when considering the eyewitness identification the court considered the
    “considerable period of time” the victim spent with her assailant and the lighting
    conditions under each observation).
    37
    Evidence, 7 Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. at 52–53 (discussing a 2004 study on
    active duty military personnel who experienced high-stress interrogations
    with real physical confrontation and low-stress interrogations without
    physical confrontation and concluding the low-stress interrogations
    produced more accurate results).
    Another commonly held belief is that in stressful situations,
    experiences become more vivid. In highly stressful conditions, vision and
    memory can be affected, resulting in significant impairments in reporting
    key characteristics of a face. Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at
    94. In stressful situations, an observer is faced with the choice to “select”
    what they are to pay attention to, and they must do so in a short window
    of time and without advance warning.
    Id. at 53.
    The noise surrounding
    the environment creates competing interest that can hijack attentional
    focus.
    Id. at 54.
      Attentional hijacking is particularly relevant when
    encountering stimuli that provoke a strong emotional response, such as
    fear and arousal.
    Id. at 55.
    Further, visual stimuli may trigger fear and
    command attention, as is the case with a weapon.
    Id. When a
    person is aware that they are perceiving a significant event,
    their attention is more focused, perception and their memory of the event
    is improved.      Henry F. Fradella, Why Judges Should Admit Expert
    Testimony on the Unreliability of Eyewitness Testimony, 2 Fed. Cts. L. Rev.
    1, 10 (2007) [hereinafter Fradella, Expert Eyewitness Testimony]. Along
    with significance, violence also impacts attention to the event.
    Id. (“Even when
    witnesses understand that they are watching a significant event, ‘the
    more violent the act, the lower the accuracy and completeness of
    perception and memory.’ This is a function of the negative impact that
    high levels of arousal and stress can produce.” (quoting Frederick Emerson
    Chemay, Unreliable Eyewitness Evidence: The Expert Psychologist and the
    38
    Defense in Criminal Cases, 
    42 La. L
    . Rev. 721, 728 (1985))). Discussing
    the Yerkes-Dodson Law, Fradella noted, “When people are concerned
    about personal safety, they tend to focus their attention on the details that
    most directly affect their safety, such as ‘blood, masks, weapons, and
    aggressive actions.’ ”
    Id. at 12
    (quoting Curt R. Bartol & Anne M. Bartol,
    Psychology and Law 221 (2d ed. 1994)). In drawing their attention towards
    what may cause harm, they focus less on details of the crime scene.
    Research has shown that the presence of a weapon during a crime
    captures the attention of witnesses and impedes their ability to attend to
    other aspects of the event, such as the face of the perpetrator. Ani A.
    Aharonian & Brian H. Bornstein, Stress and Eyewitness Memory, in
    2 Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law 770 (Brian L. Cutler ed., 2008)
    [hereinafter Aharonian & Bornstein, Stress and Eyewitness Memory]
    (“Stress effects can also be complicated by the presence of a particularly
    arousing, eye-catching aspect of the event, such as gore or a weapon.”);
    Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 93; Fradella, Expert
    Eyewitness Testimony, 2 Fed. Cts. L. Rev. at 12 (“The so-called weapons
    effect describes crime situations in which a weapon is used, and witnesses
    spend more time and psychic energy focusing on the weapon rather than
    on other aspects of the event.”); Gary L. Wells & Deah S. Quinlivan,
    Suggestive Eyewitness Identification Procedures and the Supreme Court’s
    Reliability Test in Light of Eyewitness Science: 30 Years Later, 33 Law &
    Hum. Behav. 1, 11 (2009) [hereinafter Wells & Quinlivan, Suggestive
    Eyewitness ID] (“Eyewitness experiments have consistently shown that the
    presence of a weapon . . . leads to a reduced ability to recognize the face
    of the culprit later.”). While recognizing research’s limitations 11 in how
    11Researchers are ethically limited to the amount of experimental stress that can
    be induced in a subject. Even with highly arousing materials, participants in these
    39
    stress, arousal, and recall influence one another, it does not mean that we
    must be silent on what we do know. “[I]t is clear that, overall, high levels
    of stress harm eyewitness memory in more ways than they help it.”
    Aharonian & Bornstein, Stress and Eyewitness Memory at 770.
    d. Witness characteristics and condition. An eyewitness’s ability to
    perceive and remember may be impacted by characteristics and conditions
    of the witness themselves. Personal characteristics include intoxication,
    injury, illness, age, and fatigue. 
    Lawson, 291 P.3d at 687
    ; Wells et al.,
    Eyewitness Evidence, 7 Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. at 54 (discussing how
    intoxication has been shown to correlate with lower rates of correct
    identification and how “alcohol myopia” results in less accuracy on target-
    absent conditions). Research studying recall and cognition demonstrate
    that cannabis intoxication affects memory.                Annelies Vredeveldt et al.,
    Effect of Cannabis on Eyewitness Memory: A Field Study, 32 Applied
    Cognitive Psychol. 420, 420 (2018) (discussing their study and findings of
    cannabis use on identification, recall, and confidence). From the studies,
    it appears that the effect of cannabis occurs in all stages of memory,
    however, more research is needed to determine what stage of memory
    cannabis intoxication affects the most.
    Id. at 421.
    e. Memory decay and contamination.                   Memory retrieval is the
    “process by which stored information is accessed and brought into
    consciousness, where it can be used to make decisions and guide actions.”
    Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 65.              It is a complex and
    studies are usually not personally threatened, they are bystanders rather than victims or
    potential victims. These limitations likely influence stress, behavior, degree of attention,
    and other factors that a victim of crime must undergo and process. Aharonian &
    Bornstein, Stress and Eyewitness Memory at 770; see also Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying
    the Culprit at 94 (speaking about weapon effect and recognizing that it may not be possible
    to sufficiently test the effects of stress and heightened stress in a laboratory setting due
    to limitations on participants).
    40
    dynamic system of encoding, 12 storing, 13 and remembering. 14
    Id. at 59–
    60. Memory declines over time, and once a memory is formed, it starts to
    decay. See Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 60–61; Fradella,
    Expert Eyewitness Testimony, 2 Fed. Cts. L. Rev. at 10; Gary L. Wells,
    Applied Eyewitness-Testimony Research: System Variables and Estimator
    Variables, 36 J. Personality & Soc. Psychol. 1546, 1552 (1978). The falsity
    of stable and reliable memory was addressed in 1977 by Justice Marshall.
    
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 131
    , 97 S. Ct. at 2261 (Marshall, J., dissenting) (“[T]he
    fact is that the greatest memory loss occurs within hours after an event.
    After that, the drop off continues much more slowly.”). Today, more than
    forty years later, we know even more.
    Memory can be compromised at any stage in the process. Nat’l Acad.
    of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 60. Furthermore, quality may be affected.
    Once compromised, information may “never be consolidated fully” to long-
    term memory when exposure occurs under highly emotional conditions or
    with highly emotional content.
    Id. at 61.
    Once memories are stored, there is still a possibility of modification.
    Id. at 62
    (“We forget, qualify, or distort existing memories as we acquire
    new perceptual experiences and encode new content and associations into
    memory.”).      Again, the emotional nature of the event factors into the
    storage process.
    Id. at 63–64
    (noting that highly arousing emotional
    stimuli, which tends to be more lasting than memories that are nonarousal
    12“[E]ncoding    refers to the process whereby perceived objects and events are
    initially placed into storage.” Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 60.
    13“[S]torage refers to long-term retention of the information after encoding.” Nat’l
    Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 62.
    14Remembering     refers to retrieval by which the encoded and stored information is
    brought into consciousness and is used for decision-making. Nat’l Acad. of Scis.,
    Identifying the Culprit at 65.
    41
    in stimuli, are more vivid, but are just as prone to errors and are held in
    higher confidence).
    Because of this complex system, reliability is higher when the
    identification is made within hours after the crime, and with any delay in
    time after that, reliability decreases.           Wells & Quinlivan, Suggestive
    Eyewitness ID, 33 Law & Hum. Behav. at 13 (explaining how with the
    passage of time frames measured in minutes, hours, or days, more
    memory loss occurs—described as the “forgetting curve”); see also Wise et
    al., Criminal Eyewitness Testimony, 
    42 Conn. L
    . Rev. at 505 & n.340
    (discussing the forgetting curve and retention interval).
    4. System variables.          The definition of system variables has
    broadened over time to include “factors under the control of the justice
    system that relate to (as opposed to influence) the accuracy of eyewitness
    identifications. Wells et al., Policy & Procedures, 44 Law & Hum. Behav.
    at 6. System variables were discussed in great length in Doolin, ___ N.W.2d
    at ____. Only the system variables of concern here will be discussed. 15
    15Preidentification
    instructions and blind administration will not be discussed.
    During the Booth-Harris identification, Watson was read the photographic admonition
    before each photo array was presented. The admonition stated,
    You are about to view a photographic line-up. The person who committed
    the crime may or may not be included in it. While looking at the
    photographs, keep an open mind that the individuals may not appear
    exactly as they did on the date of the crime. Their hairstyles, facial hair,
    clothing etc. may have changed. Also, photographs may not always depict
    the true complexion of a person, who may be lighter or darker than shown
    in the photo. The officer showing you the photographs has no knowledge
    of the incident. In the line-up process, the photograph will be shown to
    you one at a time and are not in any specific order. Take as much time as
    you need to look at each photograph. Even if you identify an individual
    the officer will continue to show you each photograph. The officer is not
    allowed to tell you whether your choice, if you make one, is a suspect in
    the investigation. Do not tell other witnesses that you have or have not
    identified anyone.
    Also, the two arrays were conducted under a double-blind administration since the
    administrator was not involved in the investigation and did not know the identity of the
    42
    a. Lineup construction. There is significant research and writing on
    the proper construction of a lineup or photo array. Guidance has been
    provided by the Department of Justice (DOJ), local police departments,
    and scientific research generally. The most common police-arranged tool
    for identification is the photo array. Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the
    Culprit at 23. Importantly, the suspect must not stand out from the fillers
    in the array. Third Circuit Task Force, 2019 Report of the United States
    Court of Appeals for the Third Circuit Task Force on Eyewitness
    Identifications, 92 Temp. L. Rev. 1, 35 (2019) [hereinafter Third Circuit
    Task Force, 2019 Report]; Memorandum from Sally Q. Yates, Deputy Att’y
    Gen., Dep’t of Justice, to Heads of Dep’t Law Enf’t Components, All Dep’t
    Prosecutors        (Jan. 6,   2017)     [hereinafter      Yates     Memo],       https://
    www.justice.gov/archives/opa/press-release/file/923201/download) (on
    Eyewitness Identification: Procedures for Conducting Photo Arrays,
    Procedures 3.2, 16 3.3. 17
    suspect. See Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at ___ (further discussing preidentification instructions
    and blind administration along with cited sources).
    16Fillersshould generally fit the witness’s description of the perpetrator,
    including such characteristics as gender, race, skin color, facial hair, age,
    and distinctive physical features. They should be sufficiently similar so
    that a suspect’s photograph does not stand out, but not so similar that a
    person who knew the suspect would find it difficult to distinguish him or
    her. When viewed as a whole, the array should not point to or suggest the
    suspect to the witness.
    17Where  the suspect has a unique feature, such as a scar, tattoo, or mole,
    or distinctive clothing that would make him or her stand out in a photo
    array, filler photographs should include that unique feature either by
    selecting fillers who have such a feature themselves or by altering the
    photographs of fillers to the extent necessary to achieve a consistent
    appearance.      If the suspect’s distinctive feature cannot be readily
    duplicated on the filler photographs, then the suspect’s feature can be
    blacked out and a similar black mark can be placed on the filler
    photographs. The administrator should document any alterations to
    either the fillers or the suspect’s photograph as well as the reason(s) for
    doing so.
    43
    b. Showups. 18 A showup occurs when a single suspect is shown to
    a witness, typically live, and soon following the crime. In the scientific
    community, showups, by their nature, are generally regarded as
    suggestive. Third Circuit Task Force, 2019 Report, 92 Temp. L. Rev. at 41.
    As the Supreme Court declared over fifty years ago, “It is hard to imagine
    a situation more clearly conveying the suggestion to the witness that the
    one presented is believed guilty by the police.” 
    Wade, 388 U.S. at 234
    , 87
    S. Ct. at 1936; accord Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at ___. When a witness is shown
    a single photograph, there is an increased danger of error in identifying a
    person.   See State v. Mark, 
    286 N.W.2d 396
    , 404 (Iowa 1979).                         See
    generally Amy Luria, Showup Identifications: A Comprehensive Overview of
    the Problems and a Discussion of Necessary Changes, 
    86 Neb. L
    . Rev. 515
    (2008).
    c. Repeated exposure. Research supports that repeated viewings of
    a suspect are risky due to mugshot exposure, unconscious transference,
    and source confusion. Kenneth A. Deffenbacher et al., Mugshot Exposure
    Effects: Retroactive Interference, Mugshot Commitment, Source Confusion,
    and Unconscious Transference, 30 Law & Hum. Behav. 287, 287–88
    (2006). Repeated exposure through a mugshot book, showup, or lineup,
    increases the chance of being identified in a later identification even if an
    initial identification did not occur in the first identification procedure.
    Wells et al., Policy & Procedures, 44 Law & Hum. Behav. at 25 (citing
    studies supporting the finding); see also Brandon L. Garrett, Eyewitnesses
    and Exclusion, 65 Vand. L. Rev. 451, 470 (2012) [hereinafter Garrett,
    Exclusion].
    18See   Doolin, ____ N.W.2d at ___ (discussing showups and related research).
    44
    d. Feedback to witness.            Outside information presented to the
    witness can contaminate an identification. The DOJ issued guidance on
    eyewitness       identifications    and    suggests     avoiding     “words,     sounds,
    expressions, actions[,] or behaviors” suggesting who the suspect is. Yates
    Memo, Procedure 8.1, at 5.              Should an identification be made, the
    administrator should ask for a confidence statement.
    Id. Procedure 8.2,
    at 5. If the witness provides a “vague” answer, the administrator should
    ask for the witness to provide clarification.
    Id. Procedure 8.3,
    at 5.
    Investigator or administrator feedback may result in the witness being
    more inclined to make an identification or may inflate the witness’s
    confidence in their selection.
    Id. at 8.
    e. Level of confidence. 19 With the broadening of the definition of
    system variables, confidence statements may now fall under system
    variables because of their ability to be easily contaminated by events that
    are under the control of system actors. Wells et al., Policy & Procedure, 44
    Law & Hum. Behav. at 6. Under pristine identification procedures, 20 the
    confidence that a witness expresses is usually a “highly reliable indicator
    of accuracy.” John T. Wixted & Gary L. Wells, The Relationship Between
    Eyewitness Confidence and Identification Accuracy: A New Synthesis, 18
    Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. 10, 11 (2017). Confidence correlates to pristine
    conditions, when the memory is first tested, and before contamination
    occurs.
    Id. at 13.
    19See   Doolin, ____ N.W.2d at ___ (regarding witness confidence and corresponding
    research).
    20The  pristine lineup conditions identified were (1) including only one suspect per
    lineup, (2) including a suspect that does not stand out in the lineup, (3) cautioning that
    the suspect might not be in the lineup, (4) using double-blind testing, and (5) collecting
    a confidence statement at the time of the identification. John T. Wixted & Gary L. Wells,
    The Relationship Between Eyewitness Confidence and Identification Accuracy: A New
    Synthesis, 18 Psychol. Sci. Pub. Int. 10, 20 (2017).
    45
    I now turn now to the facts of the case and apply these factors.
    B. The Flawed Identification.
    1. The limited opportunity to identify the shooter at the scene.
    During his interview at the scene with Officer Fogle, Watson stated that
    he, his cousin Deonte Carter, and Carter’s friend, Edward DeWitt, were
    talking at the park when they noticed a large group of black males standing
    on the corner of 8th and Elm. Except for Terrance Polk, Watson stated he
    did not recognize any of the males in the other group.
    The group of men continued to talk when “the next thing he knew,”
    a black male approached them.                Watson provided a very detailed
    description of the clothing the male was wearing. Watson reported the
    male was wearing a “dark colored windbreaker and a black hoodie
    underneath with the hoodie pulled up, wearing blue jeans, brown boots,
    and a black skull cap pulled over his forehead where [Watson] couldn’t see
    his face.” He did however notice something at the man’s side: a gun. After
    a brief exchange of words between the male and Carter, the male raised
    the gun from his side, hesitated, and put it back down. Carter then said
    to the man, “do what you’re gonna do,” and following those words, shooting
    began.
    Watson stated that once the shooting started he took off running
    and only returned after the shooting ended. He returned to a traumatic
    and chaotic scene, with his cousin bleeding on the ground, and called 911.
    Watson was interviewed at the scene by the officers. While he was
    not able to provide much of a physical description of the shooter, outside
    of clothing, he could provide a description of the gun, a silver-colored gun
    believed to be .40 or .45 caliber. 21      Watson was escorted to the police
    21When asked about how he estimated the caliber, Watson stated his estimation
    was based on the shell casings surrounding the scene.
    46
    station for further interviewing. In Officer Fogle’s report it is noted that he
    asked Watson “several times” if he knew the shooter and Watson stated he
    did not.
    Watson was able to provide a final description to Detective
    Schwandt, stating the shooter was a black male, about 5’11”, 150 pounds,
    twenty-one to twenty-two years old, and with a light beard. He provided
    more information about the clothing the shooter was wearing, adding the
    shooter was wearing a black hat with yellow letters. Also at this time,
    Watson admitted to smoking marijuana before arriving at the park. The
    detective could still smell the marijuana emanating from his person.
    Watson was shown a photo array soon after the shooting, and he
    did not pick anyone out as the shooter. 22 Watson was then shown a stand-
    alone picture of Booth-Harris. 23 He did not indicate that he knew Booth-
    Harris, and he did not identify him as the shooter. So even when provided
    with a one person showup, relatively shortly after the shooting, Watson
    could not make the identification.
    Based upon the events that unfolded, it is clear that there were
    troublesome estimator variables that potentially impacted his perception
    and memory of the event. The shooter was well disguised in that Watson
    stated he could not see his face. It is difficult to make a solid identification
    when one cannot see the perpetrator’s face. Further, the shooter had a
    weapon and pointed it once or twice during the short interaction, thus
    22 The police initially thought Polk was the shooter and had a picture of Polk in
    the first array shown to Watson. Watson did not identify Polk as the shooter.
    23When asked why he presented the picture of Booth-Harris to the witness,
    Detective Schwandt testified,
    Q. Why did you show him the picture then? A. Well, we just had a
    shooting in Burlington and there’s a subject with a gunshot wound. We don’t
    know if he’s a victim. We don’t know if he’s a suspect. We don’t know if he’s a
    bystander, so at that time we’re not sure what his involvement was.
    47
    triggering weapons focus. The encounter was brief. Watson stated during
    his interview two days later that everything happened quickly. It was a
    stressful and emotional event and the person who approached Watson was
    a stranger.
    The majority argues that Watson could see the shooter from the nose
    to his forehead and the description of the shooter was largely accurate with
    the   exception     of   the    height    estimate.       In    fact,   there   was    no
    contemporaneous description of the shooter.                    The descriptions came
    during the time Watson was viewing photographs.                         There was no
    description about a sharp jawline until Watson was shown the photo of
    Booth-Harris. The only description provided regarding facial features was
    the light facial hair, an observation which is inconsistent with Watson’s
    prior statement of being able to see the shooter’s nose and forehead. 24 The
    eyes were described only during the viewing of the second and third photo
    arrays. In short, in the immediate aftermath of the crime when memory
    remains fresh, Watson described the shooter’s clothes, height, and weight,
    not his face or facial features. That came later, and only when shown
    pictures by police and only after a one person showup presentation of
    Booth-Harris.
    These facts demonstrate that Watson did not have a clear view of
    the face of the perpetrator, had a very limited opportunity to view what he
    could see, and was exposed to the distraction of the presence of a weapon.
    2. The use of a highly disfavored suggestive showup. After being
    shown the array including Polk, Watson was shown a stand-alone picture
    of Booth-Harris. Detective Schwandt noted in his report that at the time
    24During the police briefing following the execution of the search warrant of Booth-
    Harris’s home, Detective Moret, the detective who lead the search, heard that the shooter
    wore “a mask covering part of his face.”
    48
    he was “advised Earl Booth-Harris [was] currently at the Monmouth
    hospital speaking to Det[ective] Tripp because he also had sustained a
    gunshot wound to the leg during the incident.”
    At the time Booth-Harris’s photo was shown, the officer was
    operating as if he may be a suspect. When asked why the photo was
    presented, the detective acknowledged that Booth-Harris may have been a
    suspect and they were trying to ascertain his role. The use of showup
    procedures calls into question subsequent identifications. “Regardless of
    how the initial misidentification comes about, the witness thereafter is apt
    to retain in his memory the image of the photograph rather than of the
    person actually seen, reducing the trustworthiness of subsequent lineup
    or courtroom identifications.” Simmons v. United States, 
    390 U.S. 377
    ,
    383–84, 
    88 S. Ct. 967
    , 971 (1968).
    Here, the use of the single picture was improper. There was no real
    urgency. Watson and the detective were at the police station, and Watson
    had already participated in one array. In Monmouth, Booth-Harris was at
    the hospital seeking medical care and was speaking to an officer. There is
    no excuse justifying the use of the highly suggestive process of a showup
    under the circumstances present.      If the investigator had the time to
    secure a photo of Booth-Harris, knowing that he could be a suspect, the
    photo could have been put into an array or lineup with fillers. See Wells
    et al., Policy & Procedures, 44 Law & Hum. Behav. at 7.
    Yet, even with the suggestive procedure, on the day of the shooting,
    when Watson’s memory was the most accurate, he could not make the
    identification.   When presented with the photo of Booth-Harris, he
    reported that he did not know who the person in the photo was.          So,
    notwithstanding the highly disfavored process, no identification was made.
    49
    But by showing a single person to Watson, an element of suggestiveness
    was injected into the identification process.
    3. The improper double arrays, with a heavy dose of suggestion. Two
    days after the shooting, when memory would have dramatically decayed,
    Watson was interviewed again by police. Sergeant McCune, who was not
    involved with the case, administered the lineup while Detective Tripp
    prepared it. Sergeant McCune read the photographic admonition before
    showing Watson the photos.
    During the first photo array, Watson lingered on the photo of Booth-
    Harris and expressed some hesitancy to say definitively that Booth-Harris
    was the shooter.   Watson expressed concern about his memory of the
    shooter’s eyes and eyebrows versus those of Booth-Harris. He expressed
    concern regarding his memory of the shooter’s height and weight versus
    those of Booth-Harris. Watson was able to say the “strong jaw structure”
    was something that he noticed in the shooter, and Booth-Harris appeared
    to have a jaw structure that reminded him of the shooter. Also, Watson
    stated that the jaw structure of the shooter was the “only thing he could
    kind of see.”   Watson concluded that array with a 50% certainty that
    Booth-Harris was the shooter, but also prefaced his 50% certainty with a
    statement to the administering officer that “[stuff] happened so quick.”
    At this point, the police did not have much.          They had no
    contemporaneous description of the shooter, got a nonidentification in a
    highly suggestive one person showup, and got a 50% confidence
    identification when Watson was presented an array with Booth-Harris.
    The police elected to attempt yet a third identification process, another
    photo array.
    In the new photo array, Watson, seeing Booth-Harris for the third
    time, identified Booth-Harris as the shooter with 70% certainty, noting
    50
    that “he doesn’t know for sure, but the [stuff] just like match, the eyeballs
    and [stuff] like that.” This response, though marginally better than the
    result in the first photo array, was hardly sufficient to overcome potential
    reasonable doubt. Rather than rest here, the police pressed on with a
    dialogue designed to push Watson to make a more positive identification.
    Sergeant McCune begins tapping repeatedly on the photo of Booth-Harris
    and exclaims “yeah.” Watson now declared, “nah, I feel like that’s him.”
    Sergeant McCune then tells Watson that “feeling like it” is more than 70%
    certainty and then asks if they are more at 100% certainty, to which
    Watson replies, “might as well say 100.” Watson is then asked to put 100%
    on the back of the photo and initial it.
    The suggestiveness here is not inconsequential. As noted by one
    commentator,     “[E]ven    fairly   minimal   confirmatory   feedback   can
    significantly inflate a witness’s assessment of her own confidence.” Keith
    A. Findley, Implementing the Lessons from Wrongful Convictions: An
    Empirical Analysis of Eyewitness Identification Reform Strategies, 
    8 Mo. L
    .
    Rev. 377, 393 (2016).      This case presents a classic example of highly
    inappropriate suggestiveness.
    It is important to note that both Watson and Booth-Harris are
    African-Americans. Research shows we are better at identifying people of
    the same racial identity.     Booth-Harris was a constant fixture among
    pictures of other black men with different skin tones, nose widths, and
    other Afrocentric features that Watson is attuned to recognizing. See Yair
    Bar-Haim et al., Nature and Nurture in Own-Race Face Processing, 17
    Psychol. Sci. 159, 160 (2006) (discussing research for own-race advantage
    and why people tend to be better at recognizing own-race faces); see also
    Doolin, ___N.W.2d at ___ (discussing more research as it relates to
    51
    Afrocentric features, recognition, and impact on the criminal justice
    system).
    Watson was exposed to Booth-Harris’s photo on three different
    occasions after the shooting. Watson arrived at 100% certainly after three
    views, one of which was a showup where no identification was made;
    another at which Watson expressed doubt and eventually landed on a 50%
    certainty identification; and finally, in a remarkable third process, where
    police inquired whether Watson’s clearly expressed 70% certainty should
    be something else, leading Watson to arrive at a 100% certainty.
    To begin with, the impact of repeated exposures is extremely
    powerful:
    A prior viewing of a suspect at an identification procedure may
    reduce the reliability of a subsequent identification procedure
    in which the same suspect is shown. A prior viewing of a
    suspect in an identification procedure raises doubts about the
    reliability of a subsequent identification procedure using the
    same suspect.
    
    Gomes, 22 N.E.3d at 916
    (emphasis omitted); see also 
    Henderson, 27 A.3d at 900
    (stating that multiple viewings of a suspect can affect later reliability
    due to risk of being unable to discern the source of recognition); 
    Lawson, 291 P.3d at 686
    –87 (same). Repeated exposure calls the identification into
    question. Watson was shown pictures of the same black male three times.
    To say Booth-Harris did not stand out is to ignore common sense and what
    science tells us. Tiffany Huinz & Kathy Pezdec, The Effect of Exposure to
    Multiple Lineups on Face Identification Accuracy, 25 Law & Hum. Behav.
    185, 195–97 (2001); John S. Shaw III & Kimberly A. McClure, Repeated
    Postevent Questioning Can Lead to Elevated Levels of Eyewitness
    Confidence, 20 Law & Hum. Behav. 629, 630–31, 649–50 (1996).
    Further, there can be no doubt that in the third identification
    process involving a photo of Booth-Harris, the exchange between Watson
    52
    and Sergeant McCune suggested to Watson that he could or should inflate
    his level of certainty from 70% to 100%. The State maintains this was not
    “encouragement” but instead an inquiry into what 70% certainty meant.
    An inquiry into what 70% certainty meant resulted in a 30% certainty
    increase and an expression of acquiescence in the form of the comment
    “might as well.” If this was the case, why was there no similar inquiry into
    ascertaining what 50% certainty meant during the first identification? And
    when does “feeling like it,” while at the same time saying, “I don’t know for
    sure,” equal 100% certainty?
    The DOJ recommends administrators avoid words, sounds,
    expressions, and actions that suggest who the suspect is. Yates Memo,
    Procedure 8.1, at 5. Once an identification has been made, a confidence
    statement should be obtained.
    Id. Procedure 8.2,
    at 5. And only if the
    statement is vague, and 70% is not vague, is the administrator to ask for
    clarification.
    Id. Procedure 8.3,
    at 5. The DOJ provides examples of how
    further exploration should be obtained. Per the DOJ, the witness should
    be asked, “You said [I think it’s #4]. What do you mean by that?”
    Id. The exemplary
    question of “what do you mean by that,” and what happened in
    practice with tapping on Booth-Harris’s picture and the statement of
    feeling like it is more than 70%, are two entirely different things. The only
    question presented was to the effect of “we’re thinking we’re more like
    100%.”
    The conduct here is beyond any clarifying question or statement and
    is an explicit question to the witness to significantly increase his certainty
    to a level that a jury could not help but place great weight in. “[T]here is
    almost nothing more convincing [to a jury] than a live human being who
    takes the stand, points a finger at the defendant, and says ‘That’s the
    one!’ ” Watkins v. Sowders, 
    449 U.S. 341
    , 352, 
    101 S. Ct. 654
    , 661 (1981)
    53
    (Brennan, J., dissenting) (quoting Elizabeth F. Loftus, Eyewitness
    Testimony 19 (1979)). Now, the State was able to say there is a witness
    who is 100% certain in his identification, when in reality he did not himself
    say he was 100% certain, but he accepted the suggestion of the officer.
    The suggestion is of 100% certainty.       Those words and numbers are
    powerful.   They were obtained in an impermissibly suggestive manner
    when the officer asked Watson if he was at 100% certainty and did not
    accept his initial answer of 70% certainty. To call this anything other than
    an encouragement to inflate his level of certainty is, at best, disingenuous.
    4. Testimony. The majority also points to the testimony of Watson
    where he states he lied about not knowing Booth-Harris.            But, the
    questioning continued beyond the portion cited in the majority opinion:
    Q. As a matter of fact, didn’t Officer Schwandt show
    you a photo lineup that day? A. Yeah. Yes.
    Q. And did you identify Earl Booth-Harris from the
    lineup? A. I think it was, like, a 70 percent chance or
    something like that, and then I think he brought some other
    pictures in or whatever.
    Q. Isn’t it a fact that he showed you Earl Booth-Harris’
    photograph all by itself apart from the lineup that day? A. I
    don’t remember. I think it was all on one paper.
    Q. But, in any event, would you agree that if [Officer
    Schwandt] [showed a photo lineup], you didn’t identify Earl
    Booth-Harris as the shooter on February 16th? A. Could you
    ask that question again.
    Q. Would you agree that you never identified Earl
    Booth-Harris as the shooter on February 16th? A. No, I did.
    Q. I’m sorry? A. I think I did, and I think I wind up
    reneging on it or something like that. I don’t know.
    Q. Are you telling us that you don’t know whether you
    identified him or not? A. The first time, like, yeah, I think I
    lied or something like that. Like, yeah, I -- I seen him, and
    then I went over the picture saying that I didn’t see him.
    54
    Watson did not say that the person he failed to identify was Booth-
    Harris. Watson was familiar with Polk and was shown an array with Polk
    on the 16th. Watson was shown an individual picture of Booth-Harris on
    the 16th. He failed to identify either of them. Watson is confusing dates
    and lineups. On the 16th, Watson was presented with a lineup involving
    Polk, not Booth-Harris. Even more problematic, the jury may have been
    lead to believe that on the day of the shooting an identification was made
    and Watson reported his certainty at 70%.
    The State argues that Watson remembered seeing the picture of
    Booth-Harris on the day of the shooting but declined to identify him
    because of fear of law enforcement. The fear of law enforcement reconciles
    with Watson lying about the gun on the day of the shooting, but not with
    his failure to identify Booth-Harris.
    If Watson declined to identify Booth-Harris on the day of the
    shooting out of fear, why two days later did he identify him with 50%
    certainty during the first lineup? Why did his certainty only jump 20%
    when he saw him for the third time and only get to 100% after speaking
    with the Sergeant?     If Watson had lied or simply failed to make the
    identification the day of the shooting, why did he not come in with 100%
    certainty after being presented with the first photo of Booth-Harris?
    III. Due Process Analysis Under the United States and Iowa
    Constitutions.
    This case involves the question of due process rights and eyewitness
    identification under both the Iowa and United States Constitutions. Under
    the Iowa Constitution, we “jealously reserve” the right to reach results
    different from the United States Supreme Court under our parallel
    provisions. State v. Ingram, 
    914 N.W.2d 794
    , 799 (Iowa 2018); see, e.g.,
    55
    Zaber v. City of Dubuque, 
    789 N.W.2d 634
    , 654 (Iowa 2010); State v.
    Wilkes, 
    756 N.W.2d 838
    , 842 n.1 (Iowa 2008).
    The Fourteenth Amendment provides that no state “shall . . . deprive
    any person of life, liberty, or property, without due process of law.” U.S.
    Const. amend. XIV. Article I, section 9 of the Iowa Constitution requires
    that “no person shall be deprived of life, liberty, or property, without due
    process of law.” Iowa Const. art. I, § 9. Our caselaw states that “[a]lthough
    the Iowa and United States Constitutions have similarly worded [due
    process] provisions, that does not mean the two regimes and the cases
    under them may be conflated.” 
    Ingram, 914 N.W.2d at 799
    .
    In Doolin, ___ N.W.2d. at ____, the United States Supreme Court
    precedent of due process and eyewitness testimony was thoroughly
    discussed by the dissent.     The Doolin dissent noted that through the
    various cases before the Court, the reliability analysis focused on “a very
    substantial likelihood of irreparable misidentification.” Id. at ___ (quoting
    
    Simmons, 390 U.S. at 384
    , 88 S. Ct. at 971). It also focused on the role of
    law enforcement and their actions in the scope of due process analysis. It
    is an incredibly forgiving standard that was created over forty years ago.
    Further in the Supreme Court analysis is the Neil v. Biggers and
    Manson framework. See Manson, 
    432 U.S. 98
    , 
    97 S. Ct. 2243
    (majority
    opinion); Neil v. Biggers, 
    409 U.S. 188
    , 
    93 S. Ct. 275
    (1972). While these
    cases somewhat considered the application of scientific evidence, these
    decisions embraced a case-by-case, multifactor analysis that was
    ultimately uninformed by science. See Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at ____ n.4;
    Wells & Quinlivan, Suggestive Eyewitness ID, 33 Law & Hum. Behav. at 5
    (noting the late 1970s is generally regarded as the birth of modern
    eyewitness research, and the work of researchers during that time did not
    appear in law reviews or other publications for legal consideration, but
    56
    instead appeared in peer-reviewed psychology journals). However, even
    before the explosive growth of science regarding eyewitness evidence, the
    role of misidentification in wrongful convictions was “well established”
    through Edwin Borchard’s 1932 book, Convicting the Innocent, where
    Borchard claimed eyewitness errors were “perhaps a major source” of
    wrongful convictions. Nicholas A. Kahn-Fogel, The Promises and Pitfalls of
    State Eyewitness Identification Reforms, 
    104 Ky. L
    . J. 99, 107 (2016)
    [hereinafter Kahn-Fogel, Promises & Pitfalls] (quoting Edwin M. Borchard,
    Convicting the Innocent: Sixty-Five Actual Errors of Criminal Justice 367
    (1932)).   Before Bouchard, in 1908, Hugo Münsterberg penned On the
    Witness Stand: Essays on Psychology and Crime where the unreliability of
    eyewitness memory was demonstrated.
    Id. at 107
    & n.43. Today we know
    even more with thousands of published and peer-reviewed studies and
    articles on eyewitness testimony. Why would we disregard them?
    The issue isn’t whether the identification would stand our current
    standard, the Biggers/ Manson test that has been determined to be
    unscientific and very forgiving. The majority’s use of Neal demonstrates
    how forgiving the standard is. See State v. Neal, 
    353 N.W.2d 83
    (Iowa
    1984).     In Neal, six days after the assault, the victim was shown
    photographs and made an identification that most closely resembled her
    assailant.
    Id. at 87.
    It wasn’t until almost two weeks later, when she was
    shown another array with the defendant, that she identified him as her
    assailant.
    Id. at 89.
      The acceptance of this identification rejects the
    science the majority states they acknowledge. In Neal, the court calls
    attention to how “even rather startling differences” between the defendant
    and other fillers in arrays have not resulted in findings of suggestiveness.
    Id. at 88.
    The current standard fails to provide adequate protections by
    rejecting system and estimator variables.
    57
    Since the time of the Manson Court, social science has demonstrated
    how “unhelpful and flawed” the Manson factors are in the proper
    evaluation of witness reliability. Garrett, Exclusion, 65 Vand. L. Rev. at
    468. This is especially troubling because the Manson Court emphasized
    that “reliability is the linchpin in determining admissibility of identification
    testimony.” 
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 114
    , 97 S. Ct. at 2253. Despite this
    extensive body of work, the Court has not revisited the Biggers/ Manson
    test for eyewitness reliability.            In Perry v. New Hampshire, Justice
    Sotomayor, dissenting, discussed the incredibly high bar set for excluding
    eyewitness identification testimony and highlighted that there has been
    one case 25 at the time of the opinion where a due process violation was
    found. 
    565 U.S. 228
    , 261, 
    132 S. Ct. 716
    , 737 (2012) (Sotomayor, J.,
    dissenting). Ultimately, the federal framework continues to ignore the
    mounting body of evidence that has “reinforced every concern [the Court’s]
    25See   Foster v. California, 
    394 U.S. 440
    , 
    89 S. Ct. 1127
    (1969). In Foster, the first
    lineup procedure involved three men: Foster, who was close to six feet tall, and two other
    men, who were approximately six inches shorter.
    Id. at 441,
    89 S. Ct. at 1128. The
    witness during the lineup was unable to identify Foster and asked to speak with him in
    a room.
    Id. After the
    one-on-one confrontation, the witness was still unable to identify
    Foster.
    Id. A week
    or ten days later, the police arranged for the same witness to
    participate in a second lineup.
    Id. This time,
    there were five men present, and Foster
    was the only repeat between the two lineups.
    Id. at 441–42,
    89 S. Ct. at 1128. After the
    second lineup, the witness was “convinced” Foster was the man.
    Id. at 442,
    89 S. Ct. at
    1128. The Court concluded the case presented “a compelling example of unfair lineup
    procedures.”
    Id. The Court
    concluded that in the first lineup, Foster stood out because
    of the contrast of heights, the jacket he was wearing (a jacket that was similar to that
    worn by the robber), and the one-on-one confrontation.
    Id. at 442–43,
    89 S. Ct. at 1128.
    Further, the Court highlighted that “[t]he practice of showing suspects singly to persons
    for the purpose of identification, and not as part of a lineup, has been widely condemned.”
    Id. at 443,
    89 S. Ct. at 1128–29 (alteration in original) (quoting Stovall v. Denno, 
    388 U.S. 293
    , 302, 
    87 S. Ct. 1967
    , 1972 (1967), abrogated on other grounds by United States v.
    Johnson, 
    457 U.S. 537
    , 
    102 S. Ct. 2579
    (1982)). After a tentative identification, another
    lineup was arranged, Foster was the only repeat in the first and second lineups, and only
    after all of this process was a definite identification produced.
    Id. The Court
    concluded
    that the suggestive procedure all but stated to the witness that “this is the man.”
    Id. at 443,
    89 S. Ct. at 1129.
    58
    precedents articulated nearly a half-century ago.”
    Id. at 565
    U.S. at 262–
    
    63, 132 S. Ct. at 738
    .
    In Booth-Harris’s motion to suppress the eyewitness testimony, he
    argued due process violations under the Fourteenth Amendment to the
    United States Constitution and article I, section 9 of the Iowa Constitution.
    Recently, in State v. Shorter, 
    893 N.W.2d 65
    (Iowa 2017), we specifically
    noted that the defendant did not raise a state constitutional challenge
    regarding the eyewitness identification evidence in his case.
    Id. at 74.
    Today we should address this issue and utilize science to take steps within
    our control to ensure due process under the Iowa Constitution when
    eyewitness evidence is proposed.
    [For] “the law will always lag behind the sciences to some
    degree because of the need for solid scientific consensus
    before the law incorporates its teachings . . . .” Appellate
    courts have a responsibility to look forward, and a legal
    concept’s longevity should not be extended when it is
    established that it is no longer appropriate.
    
    Brodes, 614 S.E.2d at 771
    (quoting 
    Long, 721 P.2d at 491
    ). If reliability is
    truly the linchpin of admissibility, we must recognize and utilize the years
    of research that have shown the Biggers/ Manson framework is unreliable
    and outdated, leading to a lack of reliability.       We have a scientific
    consensus on many issues that can be applied to safeguard due process
    rights.
    A few states, in interpreting their state constitutional due process
    requirements, have explicitly rejected the Biggers/ Manson framework,
    and this court should follow their lead.       As stated by Greg Hurley,
    Knowledge and Information Services Analyst for the National Center for
    State Courts,
    To protect the public from wrongful convictions based on an
    eyewitness misidentification, it is important that both law
    enforcement and the courts take notice of recent
    59
    developments on the issue in the social sciences. The courts
    must be aware of the malleable nature of human memory and
    the lineup practices used by law enforcement in the
    jurisdiction. Although they are downstream of the primary
    problem, the courts have the power and duty to properly
    instruct jurors, the ability to refuse to admit evidence that does
    not meet a fundamental level of trustworthiness, and the ability
    to work with justice system partners to improve the criminal
    justice system.
    Gary Hurley, Nat’l Ctr. for State Cts., Trends in State Courts: The Trouble
    with    Eyewitness     Identification    Testimony    in    Criminal    Cases,
    https://www.ncsc.org/microsites/trends/home/Monthly-Trends-
    Articles/2017/The-Trouble-with-Eyewitness-Identification-Testimony-in-
    Criminal-Cases.aspx (emphasis added).
    States are interpreting their due process clauses and are modifying
    how eyewitness identification testimony is used in trial. See 
    Young, 374 P.3d at 412
    –28 (holding the Manson test does not adequately protect due
    process rights under the Alaska Constitution and utilizing scientific
    evidence in adopting a new approach); State v. Harris, 
    191 A.3d 119
    , 123,
    133–45 (Conn. 2018) (reaffirming the due process clause under the state
    constitution provides greater protection than the Federal Constitution and
    utilizing other state precedent of estimator and system variables);
    Commonwealth v. Johnson, 
    650 N.E.2d 1257
    , 1260, 1261 (Mass. 1995)
    (rejecting the Manson reliability test as an accurate interpretation of the
    state’s due process clause); State v. Adams, 
    423 N.E.2d 379
    , 383–84 (N.Y.
    1981) (rejecting Manson test under the state’s constitution); cf. State v.
    Lujan, 
    459 P.3d 992
    , 1000–05 (Utah 2020) (utilizing scientific research in
    determining admissibility of eyewitness testimony under the Utah Rules of
    Evidence while reserving the possibility that the Utah due process clause
    may differ from the federal standard).
    The majority cites State v. Roberson, 
    935 N.W.2d 813
    (Wis. 2019),
    and their about-face in utilizing eyewitness science in determining
    60
    admissibility. The Roberson opinion focuses attention on social science
    and the role of social science in the application and interpretation of law.
    I agree with that assertion to a degree. Social science is characterized as
    a branch of science dealing with human behavior in its social and cultural
    aspects. Should courts use the social science that was utilized to endorse
    the theory of racial inferiority that was the catalyst for racist policy and
    judicial decisions? No. We must also recognize that the perception or
    “science” of inferiority had no scientific basis. Social science that helped
    shape racially discriminatory policy, as mentioned in Roberson, was based
    on physical characteristics and if someone was “well-born.”
    Id. at 821–22.
    In recognizing that use of social science in judicial decision-making, we
    must also attribute social science’s contributions to socially important
    decisions. See
    id. at 834
    (Dallet, J., dissenting) (citing decisions leading
    to the decriminalization of consensual same-sex intimate conduct and the
    abolition of the death penalty against the mentally ill and juveniles).
    Today, however, social science of societal beliefs is not the science I
    would rely upon.        Eyewitness science utilizes studies that have a
    foundation in neuroscience and other disciplines, from studies that are
    reliable. Missing from both the Roberson opinion and today’s majority is
    the research that calls into question the validity or reliability of the
    information presented, through methodologies that are tested and retested
    through various other methodologies.       The science is sound and the
    wrongful convictions resulting from misidentification provide support to
    the notion that there is a problem with the way eyewitness identification
    testimony is handled.
    This isn’t the “social science . . . embod[ying] the subjective beliefs
    of the time.”
    Id. at 822
    (majority opinion). This isn’t the same type of
    science that reinforced the beliefs and findings of Plessy v. Ferguson,
    61
    antimiscegenation laws, and forced sterilizations. See Plessy v. Ferguson,
    
    163 U.S. 537
    , 
    16 S. Ct. 1138
    (1896), overruled by Brown v. Bd. of Educ.,
    
    347 U.S. 483
    , 495, 
    74 S. Ct. 686
    , 692 (1954). The science of eyewitness
    identification is not rooted in the subjective belief of superiority or
    inferiority, and this is not a societal value. It is based in how the brain
    perceives, how the brain recalls, and how the brain processes. In Justice
    Dallet’s dissent, she stated that the majority, by abrogating State v.
    Dubose, 
    699 N.W.2d 582
    (Wis. 2005), “erodes the due process protection
    afforded by the Wisconsin Constitution and places jurors in the impossible
    position of separating the taint of a suggestive single photo identification
    from its reliability.” 
    Roberson, 935 N.W.2d at 831
    (Dallet, J., dissenting).
    Under the current Biggers/ Manson factors, we are asked to balance
    the corrupting effects of a suggestive identification procedure against
    reliability factors that decades of research have indicated are unreliable.
    This practice is harmful to those who encounter our criminal justice
    system and does little to deter suggestive practices engaged in by law
    enforcement.   Federally, “[t]he development of due process protections
    against mistaken identification evidence, begun in Stovall, was continued
    in Simmons” and has effectively ended in the Manson framework. 
    Manson, 432 U.S. at 121
    , 97 S. Ct. at 2256 (Marshall, J., dissenting). “But, the
    Federal Constitution merely sets a ‘constitutional floor’ below which state
    constitutional interpretations may not sink.” Mark S. Cady, The Vanguard
    of Equality: The Iowa Supreme Court’s Journey to Stay Ahead of the Curve
    on an Arc Bending Towards Justice, 76 Albany L. Rev. 1991, 1992 (2013).
    Iowa has a history of protecting those within our borders before
    federal courts reached the opposite or same conclusion. See 
    Varnum, 763 N.W.2d at 906
    –07 (finding same-sex marriage equality six years before
    62
    Obergefell26); Coger v. Nw. Union Packet Co., 
    37 Iowa 145
    , 153–54 (1873)
    (providing equality in public accommodations twenty years before Plessy27
    arrived at the separate but equal doctrine); Clark v. Bd. of Dirs., 
    24 Iowa 266
    , 274 (1868) (desegregating public schools over eighty years before the
    Brown 28 decision); In re Ralph, 
    1 Morris 1
    (1839) (extending equal
    protection to all men, regardless of color, and eighteen years before the
    Supreme Court reached the opposite conclusion in the Dred Scott29
    decision).
    In Wade, the Court called attention to the “annals of criminal law
    [being] rife with instances of mistaken identification,” of which we know
    now more than 
    ever. 388 U.S. at 228
    , 87 S. Ct. at 1933. The Manson
    factors   “are      flatly   contradicted     by   well-respected      and    essentially
    unchallenged empirical studies,” and the “time has come for a more
    empirically sound approach.” State v. Ramirez, 
    817 P.2d 774
    , 780 (Utah
    1991) (quoting 
    Long, 721 P.2d at 491
    –92), abrogated by 
    Lujan, 459 P.3d at 999
    .
    To provide due process, we must adjust and incorporate what we
    know to best facilitate a system that is fair and seeks justice. We simply
    cannot dismiss this expansive body of research. The Iowa Constitution
    provides a foundation for society and our interpretation fosters growth
    “consistent with the increasing knowledge and understanding of the
    world.” Mark S. Cady, A Pioneer’s Constitution: How Iowa’s Constitutional
    History Uniquely Shapes Our Pioneering Tradition in Recognizing Civil
    26Obergefell   v. Hodges, 576 U.S. ___, ___, 
    135 S. Ct. 2584
    , 2608 (2015).
    27Plessy,   
    163 U.S. 537
    .
    28Brown,  
    347 U.S. 483
    , 
    74 S. Ct. 686
    , supplemented sub nom., 
    349 U.S. 294
    , 
    75 S. Ct. 753
    (1955).
    29Dred   Scott v. Sandford, 60 U.S. (19 How.) 393 (1857), superseded by U.S. Const.
    amend. XIV.
    63
    Rights and Civil Liberties, 60 Drake L. Rev. 1133, 1142 (2012). So the
    question is, what increased knowledge have we gained?
    This is what we know.       Eyewitness identification evidence is a
    leading cause of wrongful convictions, and mistaken identifications
    contributed to approximately 71% of the more than 360 wrongful
    convictions subsequently overturned by DNA evidence to date in the
    United States.    Innocence Project, Eyewitness Identification Reform,
    https://www.innocenceproject.org/eyewitness-identification-reform/
    [https://perma.cc/J2TA-5BHZ].      As devastating as these numbers are,
    they only tell us the number of those wrongfully convicted because DNA
    evidence was available to later testing.     For many, there is no DNA
    evidence, whether the evidence is not preserved or the sample is not
    sufficient enough to test. The statistics only give us a snapshot of the
    wrongful convictions that occur in the United States.      There are likely
    countless others who remain incarcerated or have faced a term of
    incarceration because DNA evidence was not available.
    With all the information available about how sweeping this problem
    is and how, as a system, we can address the problem, we choose not to
    act. We perpetuate the illusion of due process and protection for those
    involved in the criminal justice system, and in doing so, we do nothing to
    curb the unacceptably high risk of wrongful convictions. Common sense
    is losing out to precedent, and convictions are held in a higher regard than
    the pursuit of truth.
    In Doolin, I concluded that under the Iowa Constitution’s due
    process clause, the wisest path forward regarding in-court identifications
    should a per se exclusion approach. Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at ___. First-time,
    in-court identifications would be inadmissible absent a prior identification
    made through nonsuggestive procedures.
    Id. In arriving
    at a per se
    64
    exclusion approach, I surveyed various states and what they are doing to
    combat unreliable eyewitness testimony. There, I discussed Harris, 
    191 A.3d 119
    .       Under the Iowa due process clause, for eyewitness
    identification, I would adopt a methodology similar to that created under
    Harris.
    In Harris, the Supreme Court of Connecticut determined that,
    although the defendant’s due process challenge under the Federal
    Constitution fell short in light of traditional caselaw, the due process
    clause under the Connecticut Constitution required 
    more. 191 A.3d at 123
    .   While the court in Harris ruled the admission of the eyewitness
    identification testimony to be harmless error, the Harris court outlined a
    new science-based approach in determining admissibility under their state
    due process clause.
    Id. at 143.
    The Harris court developed a procedural framework to consider
    eyewitness identification challenges.       Finding the Biggers factors
    “insufficiently protective” of due process rights under their state
    constitution, the Harris court adopted a different due process framework.
    Id. at 133–43.
       Under the new framework, a defendant may obtain a
    pretrial hearing where the defendant carries the initial burden of offering
    “some evidence that a system variable undermined the reliability of the
    eyewitness identification.”
    Id. at 143.
      The burden then shifts to the
    prosecution to show that the identification was reliable, accounting for all
    relevant estimator and system variables.
    Id. If the
    prosecution meets its
    burden, the burden shifts back to the defendant to prove a “very
    substantial likelihood of misidentification” in order for the evidence to be
    excluded.
    Id. The defendant
    was entitled to challenge the eyewitness identification
    under the Harris framework. Based on the record in this case, I cannot
    65
    see how the eyewitness identification would have been admitted under
    Harris standards. Because the district court failed to utilize the Harris
    framework, I would reverse Booth-Harris’s conviction and remand the case
    for retrial.
    IV. Ineffective Assistance of Counsel: Use of the ISBA Model
    Jury Instruction.
    A. Introduction. The jury was instructed using the ISBA Model
    Jury Instruction on eyewitness identification. Booth-Harris argues that
    his counsel should have requested jury instructions incorporating well-
    established system and estimator variables and that failure to do so
    constituted ineffective assistance of counsel. In addressing this claim, I
    begin by examining the history of the ISBA Model Jury Instruction 200.45.
    B. History of ISBA Model Jury Instruction 200.45. The current
    ISBA instruction regarding eyewitness identification testimony is derived
    from United States v. Telfaire, 
    469 F.2d 552
    (D.C. Cir. 1972) (per curiam).
    In Telfaire, the court commented on the “one witness rule” 30 and its role
    in Anglo-American jurisprudence.
    Id. at 554.
    Telfaire also commented on
    the power of the presumption of innocence and the adversarial system with
    safeguards to “dilute the danger of conviction of the innocent,” a problem
    of concern for “every civilized system of justice.”
    Id. at 554–55
    (“The
    presumption of innocence that safeguards the common law system must
    be a premise that is realized in instruction and not merely a promise.”).
    In pursuit of that promise, the court recognized the importance for
    a special instruction with eyewitness identification emphasizing that
    eyewitness identification testimony involves special and heightened
    30The one-witness rule recognizes that some crimes are solitary and allows for a
    case to be sent to a jury, and a verdict to be upheld, on the uncorroborated testimony of
    a single witness, and the witness need not be a victim. See Strickland v. United States,
    
    332 A.2d 746
    , 749 (D.C. 1975).
    66
    problems of reliability.
    Id. at 555.
    The purpose of the instruction was to
    emphasize to the jury the importance and need to find the identification
    convincing beyond a reasonable doubt.
    Id. For jurors
    to find that, the
    Telfaire court embraced education as a means of achieving this goal.
    Id. at 557.
    In Chief Judge Bazelon’s concurrence, he recognized that the
    instructions went far in providing illumination to the shortcomings and
    pitfalls of eyewitness identification testimony, though he believed the
    instructions did not go far enough.
    Id. at 559
    (Bazelon, C.J., concurring).
    Specifically, he called attention to the issues that arise in cross-racial
    identification, an issue not addressed in the special instructions.
    Id. Utilizing data,
    Chief Judge Bazelon called attention to the “widely held
    commonsense view” that cross-racial identification faces greater difficulty
    than same-race identification.
    Id. He recognized
    the danger as just “as
    relevant to the accuracy” of identification as other factors accounted for in
    the model instructions.
    Id. at 560.
    Judge Leventhal authored a concurrence to address Chief Judge
    Bazelon’s concerns regarding cross-racial identification.
    Id. at 561–63
    (Leventhal, J., concurring). Judge Leventhal expressed concern that the
    issue of cross-race identification had not been developed enough to be
    addressed in the model instructions.
    Id. at 561–62
    (“The issue of inter-
    racial identifications is not ripe for this kind of distillation of wisdom
    involving as it does a matter on which there is only ‘meager data’ and an
    assertion of ‘common sense’ views that merit further consideration.”). In
    Judge Leventhal’s view, the issue was a launching point to be used to
    identify a problem and, if needed, could warrant further discussion if it
    was an issue in a specific case.
    Id. at 563.
                                         67
    C. Impact of Science on Eyewitness Identification Instruction.
    The Telfaire instructions were an “influential set of model jury
    instructions” to be used in appropriate cases involving eyewitness
    identification testimony. Nat’l Acad. of Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 41.
    These instructions added factors for the jury to consider in assessing
    eyewitness testimony.    
    Telfaire, 469 F.2d at 558
    –59 (majority opinion)
    (outlining the model special instruction on identification); Nat’l Acad. of
    Scis., Identifying the Culprit at 41–42. Some states adopted the cautionary
    instructions, and Iowa was one that did. See Iowa State Bar Ass’n, Iowa
    Criminal Jury Instructions 200.45 (2018).
    However, the Telfaire instructions fail to provide guidance on
    variables that reduce the reliability of identification procedures and ignore
    the issue of estimator and system variables that have since been identified
    as important reliability factors. See Kahn-Fogel, Promises & Pitfalls, 
    104 Ky. L
    .J. at 118–19. The instructions are still based on what was known in
    the 1970s. The instructions provide cautionary statements about some
    generalities regarding the witness’s opportunity and capacity to view the
    perpetrator and identification procedure.
    Id. at 119–20;
    Telfaire, 469 F.2d
    at 558
    –59. Studies have shown the Telfaire instructions were ineffective
    safeguards against misidentification. Kahn-Fogel, Promises & Pitfalls, 
    104 Ky. L
    .J. at 119.
    There is no doubt that the Telfaire jury instructions are no longer
    adequate in light of the scientific developments. Examples of scientifically
    based instructions may be found in 
    Henderson, 27 A.3d at 894
    –912, 925–
    26, and 
    Gomes, 22 N.E.3d at 907
    –17.         There is no one version of an
    instruction that must be used in all cases, but I think it is abundantly
    clear that a criminal defendant, in cases where eyewitness identification is
    68
    involved, is entitled to a science-based instruction such as those presented
    in Henderson and Gomes.
    D. Ineffective Assistance of Counsel. On appeal, Booth-Harris
    asserts that his trial counsel was ineffective for failing to request
    instructions   incorporating    well-established   scientific   information
    regarding system and estimator variables.      Accordingly, the merits are
    analyzed through Strickland v. Washington, 
    466 U.S. 668
    , 
    104 S. Ct. 2052
    (1984).
    The first prong requires an examination of whether trial counsel fell
    below a level of competence expected for a similar attorney. The question
    is whether a “normally competent attorney could have concluded that the
    question . . . was not worth raising.” State v. Fountain, 
    786 N.W.2d 260
    ,
    266 (Iowa 2010) (quoting State v. Schoelerman, 
    315 N.W.2d 67
    , 72 (Iowa
    1982)). A competent lawyer must stay abreast of legal developments to
    render effective assistance of counsel. Id.; see also Doolin, ___ N.W.2d at
    ___ (citing the Iowa Rules of Professional Conduct and how they require
    maintaining competence in changing and evolving fields).
    Based on the above information, the eyewitness identification
    evidence controversy has been in the public and legal purview for over forty
    years. In Telfaire, Chief Judge Bazelon stated, “The jury’s knowledge of
    the relevant factors should not turn on the inadvertence or inexperience
    of trial counsel, and this is particularly so where the issue of identity is
    the question of guilt or innocence.” 
    Telfaire, 469 F.2d at 560
    (Bazelon,
    C.J., concurring). The accuracy of eyewitness identification testimony is
    routinely overestimated by jurors and the confidence of the identification
    often carries great weight with the juror. 
    Perry, 565 U.S. at 263
    –64, 132
    S. Ct. at 738–39. It falls to defense counsel to be well-informed and active
    in protecting their client’s due process rights through knowledge of these
    69
    well-known and established fallacies of eyewitness identification and to
    inform the jury on both the best practices and pitfalls of eyewitness
    identification. Gomes, Henderson, and Lawson, to name a few cases, have
    addressed these issues at great length. Each case was also accompanied
    by either a special report or an appendix of scholarly work addressing the
    issues in question. 
    Gomes, 22 N.E.3d at 918
    –27; 
    Henderson, 27 A.3d at 894
    –912; 
    Lawson, 291 P.3d at 695
    & n.10.
    Further, a veritable library on the question of science and eyewitness
    identification can be quickly developed by any lawyer through a computer
    search of commonly available legal databases. A search of “eyewitness
    identification” in the same sentence as “science” produces rich results with
    hundreds of hits, reflecting reliable, scholarly secondary literature of
    germane materials. Or, a search of “eyewitness identification” in the same
    sentence as “instruction” produces, again, hundreds of results, again,
    many highly germane. The information regarding eyewitness identification
    instructions is not stored in some kind of heavily guarded legal Fort Knox,
    or encrypted in some complicated and remote file of the national
    intelligence directorate.   It is readily available to any lawyer with a
    modicum of curiosity, a mouse, and a few minutes time. There should be
    no lawyer practicing criminal law in the State of Iowa without a general
    knowledge of recent developments in the law and science of eyewitness
    identification. A lawyer without such knowledge has no place in an Iowa
    courtroom defending clients facing deprivation of liberty where eyewitness
    identifications are an important part of the State’s case.
    The remaining question is whether the failure to give a science-
    based instruction in this case caused prejudice. A review of the record
    makes it abundantly clear that Watson’s identification was a critical part
    of the State’s case. An appropriate eyewitness instruction would have
    70
    significantly enhanced the ability of the defense to challenge the credibility
    of Watson’s identification, assuming it was admissible, and empowered the
    jury to more accurately assess the credibility of the identification.
    Because of the weight jurors give eyewitness identification, it is
    imperative that the jury be instructed on the vital issues surrounding
    eyewitness identification. For example, a science-based instruction would
    have told the jury that human memory does not function like a camera;
    that a witness’s expressed certainty, standing alone, may not indicate the
    accuracy of the identification; and that a prior viewing of a suspect at an
    identification procedure may reduce the reliability of any subsequent
    identification procedure. See 
    Gomes, 22 N.E.3d at 918
    –27. Further, a
    science-based instruction would have educated the jury about estimator
    variables and would have, for example, (a) identified the issue of the
    disguise and how disguises affect a witness’s ability to identify a person,
    (b) advised that an exposure of short duration limits the power of memory,
    (c) identified the distraction of weapons focus as a factor undermining the
    accuracy of the identification, (d) brought up the possible effects of
    Watson’s intoxication on identification, (e) illuminated the importance of
    lack of familiarity with the suspect, (f) explored the role of memory decay
    in the accuracy of eyewitness identification, and (g) explained the risks of
    misidentification through contaminating suggestion.
    The jury did not receive the information it needed regarding system
    and estimator variables so they could properly assess the weight of the
    evidence provided. The lack of a science-based eyewitness identification
    instruction in this case undermines my confidence in the verdict and is
    sufficient to satisfy the prejudice prong of an ineffective-assistance-of-
    counsel claim. See Commonwealth v. Pressley, 
    457 N.E.2d 1119
    , 1120–
    71
    21 (Mass. 1983) (reversing conviction based on failure to give adequate
    eyewitness identification instruction).
    V. Conclusion.
    The majority opinion “persist[s] in wholesale reliance on an archaic
    test based on seemingly logical assumptions that have since been refuted.”
    Small v. State, 
    211 A.3d 236
    , 256 (Md. 2019) (Barbera, C.J., concurring).
    They welcome a change in model jury instructions if the ISBA accepts the
    invitation. In the meantime, criminal defendants face conviction by juries
    that are woefully ill informed. The New Jersey Supreme Court put a stop
    to this kind of process in 2011.          See 
    Henderson, 27 A.3d at 918
    .
    Massachusetts has done so as well. See 
    Gomes, 22 N.E.3d at 917
    –18. We
    should do the same today.
    For all of the above reasons, I would reverse the conviction and
    remand the case.