United States v. Clark ( 2015 )


Menu:
  •                UNITED STATES NAVY-MARINE CORPS
    COURT OF CRIMINAL APPEALS
    WASHINGTON, D.C.
    Before
    F.D. MITCHELL, J.A. FISCHER, M.C. HOLIFIELD
    Appellate Military Judges
    UNITED STATES OF AMERICA
    v.
    DUSTIN M. CLARK
    AIRMAN (E-3), U.S. NAVY
    NMCCA 201400232
    GENERAL COURT-MARTIAL
    Sentence Adjudged: 21 February 2014.
    Military Judge: CDR Robert P. Monahan, Jr., JAGC, USN.
    Convening Authority: Commandant, Naval District Washington,
    Washington Navy Yard, Washington, DC.
    Staff Judge Advocate's Recommendation: LCDR J.D. Pilling,
    JAG, USN.
    For Appellant: David Sheldon, Esq.; Capt Michael Magee,
    USMC.
    For Appellee: LT Jetti Gibson, JAGC, USN; Capt Matthew M.
    Harris, USMC.
    14 July 2015
    ---------------------------------------------------
    OPINION OF THE COURT
    ---------------------------------------------------
    THIS OPINION DOES NOT SERVE AS BINDING PRECEDENT, BUT MAY BE CITED AS
    PERSUASIVE AUTHORITY UNDER NMCCA RULE OF PRACTICE AND PROCEDURE 18.2.
    PER CURIAM:
    A military judge sitting as a general court-martial
    convicted the appellant, contrary to his pleas, of one
    specification each of rape and forcible sodomy, in violation of
    Articles 120 and 125, Uniform Code of Military Justice, 10
    U.S.C. §§ 920 and 925. 1 The military judge sentenced the
    appellant to seven years’ confinement, reduction to pay grade E-
    1, and a dishonorable discharge. The convening authority
    approved the sentence as adjudged and, except for the punitive
    discharge, ordered it executed.
    The appellant raises two assignments of error (AOE):
    (1) his convictions are legally and factually insufficient and
    (2) his trial defense counsel were ineffective in failing to
    compel discovery of the victim’s mental health records.
    After carefully considering the record of trial and the
    submissions of the parties, we find merit in the appellant’s
    first AOE asserting that the evidence is factually insufficient
    to sustain his convictions. We take action in our decretal
    paragraph. 2 Arts. 59(a) and 66(c), UCMJ.
    Background
    On the evening of Saturday 24 March 2012, Ms. SW
    accompanied her friend, Petty Officer AM, to a house party on
    the military installation where AM was stationed. After
    consuming alcohol and socializing at the party, SW, AM, and
    several other party goers accompanied the appellant to his
    house, located on the same military installation, to continue
    socializing.
    At trial, SW testified that throughout the evening she
    engaged in consensual amorous activity with three different men,
    including the appellant, before ultimately “blacking out” while
    at the appellant’s house. At the initial party, SW and AM went
    into a bedroom where, according to SW’s testimony, she and AM
    laid together in bed for about fifteen minutes and kissed
    “probably briefly.” 3 This encounter ended when another party
    goer, Mr. WC, interrupted and asked them to rejoin the party
    downstairs. 4 Shortly thereafter, SW and WC 5 went to an upstairs
    1
    The military judge acquitted the appellant of one specification of
    aggravated sexual assault for engaging in a sexual act with a person who was
    substantially incapacitated. The rape and aggravated sexual assault
    specifications were pled in the alternative.
    2
    AOE 2 is rendered moot by our actions.
    3
    Record at 224-25.
    4
    
    Id. at 168.
    2
    bedroom where they consensually participated in sexual activity
    short of intercourse. 6 This encounter ended when WC retreated to
    a nearby bathroom to vomit due to his alcohol consumption. A
    witness testified to seeing WC come out of the bedroom and head
    to the bathroom and that WC was naked and wearing a condom. 7 AM
    testified that he saw SW in the bedroom after WC left for the
    bathroom and she was in her underwear and getting dressed. 8
    A group including SW, AM, and the appellant then left the
    party and went to the appellant’s house. SW testified that
    while at the appellant’s house, she and the appellant “made out”
    while sitting on the couch in the living area. 9 Other witnesses
    reported seeing SW and the appellant mutually kissing while
    seated on the couch. 10 SW testified that at the end of the
    evening she accepted the appellant’s offer to spend the night at
    his home because she was too intoxicated to drive. SW testified
    that the next morning she awoke in an upstairs room, completely
    naked and on the floor, next to the appellant who was also
    naked. 11 SW testified that at this point her last clear memory
    was of going upstairs with the appellant. 12
    According to SW, she then left the appellant’s house,
    retrieved her car from the site of the original party, and drove
    off the base. SW testified that she got lost while attempting
    to drive home, so she stopped and slept in her car for several
    more hours. After she awoke, SW went to a friend’s house where
    she spent the remainder of her Sunday. 13
    SW testified that on Monday afternoon, after work, she
    noticed bruises on her thighs. 14 Still unable to recall events
    5
    WC testified that he is six-foot four inches tall and weighs two hundred and
    thirty pounds. 
    Id. at 507.
    6
    
    Id. at 169,
    229-34, 509-10.
    7
    
    Id. at 401.
    8
    
    Id. at 385.
    9
    
    Id. at 178-79.
    10
    
    Id. at 364,
    386, 399-400.
    11
    
    Id. at 188-89.
    12
    
    Id. at 190.
    13
    
    Id. at 190-94.
    3
    from Saturday night, SW took photos of the bruises and then went
    to a local hospital to have a sexual assault exam performed. 15
    The exam results proved inconclusive as to whether SW had
    engaged in intercourse. 16 SW was at the hospital from Monday
    night until early Tuesday morning.
    SW testified that on Tuesday she began to have
    recollections of what happened Saturday night. SW described
    having four segmented memories of what occurred with the
    appellant that night. In further clarifying her recollections
    SW testified, “[s]egmented, just like there’s spaces of time in
    between them that I have absolutely no recollection of what
    happened. I don’t remember the specific order of--of
    occurrences.” 17 SW then testified to recalling the following
    “segmented” memories:
    (1)     She was clothed and lying on her back on the floor and
    appellant was on top of her and holding her arms down. She
    also testified to recalling feeling pressure on her legs,
    but she could not specifically recall how the appellant was
    positioned. She testified that she resisted and asked the
    appellant to stop, but she “gave up pretty quickly” because
    she was intoxicated and scared. She did not testify to
    what, if anything, the appellant was doing to her in
    addition to holding her in this position; 18
    (2)     She was completely naked on her back and the appellant was
    on top of her and penetrating her vagina with his penis.
    She testified that she recalled it being painful. She
    could not recall whether the appellant was clothed or
    unclothed at this time. Additionally, she could not recall
    whether the appellant was restraining her arms and did not
    testify to the appellant restraining her in any fashion or
    to any communication between her and the appellant at this
    point; 19
    14
    
    Id. at 201.
    15
    
    Id. at 209.
    16
    
    Id. at 349.
    17
    
    Id. at 183.
    18
    
    Id. at 183-85.
    4
    (3)     She was on her back and the appellant turned her over by
    the hips from her back to her front. SW did not testify as
    to her or the appellant’s state of dress at this time,
    whether she resisted the appellant’s actions or whether
    they engaged in any communication; 20
    (4)     She was lying on her back facing upward and the appellant
    used his hand to open her mouth and insert his penis. She
    provided no information as to what, if anything, she did to
    resist the appellant’s actions. Nor did she testify to the
    amount of force the appellant used to open her mouth. She
    could not testify to the appellant’s physical position
    during this event, but recalls that she gagged when he
    inserted his penis in her mouth. 21
    SW also testified to a memory of the appellant sucking and
    biting her breasts, 22 however she did not clarify whether this
    was part of one of aforementioned segmented memories or
    separate. She testified that none of this sexual activity with
    the appellant was consensual. 23 She further testified that while
    “making out” with the appellant on the couch earlier that night,
    she told him she was not interested in having sex with him. 24
    Approximately three months later, SW reported that she had
    been raped to law enforcement personnel. She testified that she
    did so following advice from her therapist that reporting the
    incident was a better course of action than her plan to confront
    the appellant directly. SW testified, however, that her primary
    motive in going to law enforcement was to do all she could to
    protect others from the appellant. 25
    19
    
    Id. at 185-86.
    20
    
    Id. at 186.
    21
    
    Id. at 187-88.
    22
    
    Id. at 187.
    23
    
    Id. at 189.
    24
    
    Id. at 179.
    25
    
    Id. at 212-13.
                                       5
    The appellant provided two sworn statements to Naval
    Criminal Investigative Service (NCIS) investigating agents. 26 In
    his initial statement the appellant confirmed that he met SW
    when she and others came to his house on the night in question.
    However, the appellant denied that she spent the night at his
    house and further denied engaging in any sexual activity with
    her. In his second statement, given approximately five months
    later, the appellant stated that he blacked out that night and
    awoke the next morning alone on the floor of his room wearing
    only his boxers and with a condom lying next to him. He further
    stated that he thought, at that moment, that he’d had sex with
    SW because she was the only woman at his home the prior night.
    He indicated that he felt ashamed at that time because, although
    he and his wife had recently separated, he was still married.
    Expert Testimony
    Dr. Stafford Henry, M.D., was called as an expert witness 27
    by the Government and provided the following testimony:
    TC    Doctor, are you familiar with the phrase “alcohol-
    induced blackout”?
    WIT   I am.
    TC    Can you tell the military judge what is that.
    WIT   Sure. An alcohol-induced blackout is a--it is a form
    of amnesia. Amnesia is basically a lack of memory.
    It is an antegrade amnesia. It is an amnesia which is
    causally linked to the self-administration of alcohol.
    MJ    Doctor, what’s the meaning of the term “antegrade”?
    WIT   Judge, there are—there are two kinds of amnesia,
    retrograde and antegrade. Antegrade amnesia is an
    amnesia, for the purposes of this hearing, of what
    occurred during a period of intoxication. Retrograde
    amnesia would be biographical information such as
    where you went to school, what your mother’s maiden
    name is. So antegrade means from—from—from—to one
    point forward. Retrograde means historical.
    MJ    I understand.        You may proceed.
    26
    Prosecution Exhibits 1 & 2.
    27
    Dr. Henry testified that he is board certified in general psychiatry,
    forensic psychiatry and addiction psychiatry and he was recognized by the
    court as an expert witness in those fields.
    6
    TC    Doctor, are there different types of blackout?
    WIT   Yes, there are.
    TC    And what are they?
    WIT   There are two kinds of blackouts. One is fragmentary,
    which is more common. The second is en bloc …
    TC    Can you describe the difference between a fragmentary
    and an en bloc.
    WIT   Sure. Essentially, Judge, en bloc blackout is a—it’s
    an antegrade amnesia----
    MJ    I’m sorry, we’re speaking of en bloc right now or
    fragmentary?
    WIT   En bloc.
    MJ    Okay, I understand. Please proceed.
    WIT   An en bloc is an antegrade amnesia with generally a
    very discrete beginning and a very discrete end. So
    there’s a block of time for which that person cannot
    recall. Alternatively, a fragmentary blackout is just
    that. It is a recollection of events which occurred
    during the period of intoxication which is partial.
    TC    Can alcohol cause fragmentary blackouts?
    WIT   Yes, alcohol can cause fragmentary blackouts.
    TC    Is there a set or required BAC or amount of alcohol
    one would have to consume in order to experience some
    type of blackout?
    WIT   No.
    TC    At what point would the—would an individual who
    experienced a blackout realize that they experienced a
    blackout? More specifically, while a person is in a
    state that they later will not recall, does that
    individual know that they are in a blackout?
    WIT   You only know you’re in a blackout retrospectively.
    Only—it is only after the fact, after—after—at some
    point later that you realize you do not have a
    recollection for a past event.
    TC    And then can a person walk and talk and then later not
    have memory of that walking and talking?
    WIT   That is possible.
    7
    MJ    That’s within the context of an alcohol-induced
    blackout?
    WIT   Absolutely, sir.
    MJ    You may continue.
    TC    So is it possible that an individual could—could be
    somewhat functioning, moving, communicating, but then
    later have no recollection of that due to a
    fragmentary blackout?
    WIT   Yes, that is possible. 28
    Dr. Henry further testified on direct examination that he
    thoroughly reviewed the investigations and medical information
    in this case and he extensively interviewed SW. Dr. Henry
    testified that, in his professional opinion, SW “provided a
    description which was very clinically consistent with a
    fragmentary blackout.” 29
    During the defense case in chief, Dr. Thomas Grieger, M.D.,
    was called as an expert in the fields of clinical and forensic
    psychiatry. Dr. Grieger testified extensively regarding the
    formation of memories and the potential effect of alcohol on
    memory retention. Dr. Grieger provided the following testimony:
    DC    Okay. Can you describe the mechanism of a fragmentary
    blackout, what—what that means in terms of memory.
    WIT   Yeah, what—what a fragmentary blackout is is that you
    are putting portions of an experience into short-term—
    into long-term memory as that event is occurring. It
    can also--it can often be the most salient aspects of
    something, the most significant aspects of something
    that’s going on, the most emotional aspects of
    something that’s going on, but you’re not really
    putting into long-term memory all of the details that
    go between those salient events. So the next day you
    would recall the salient events and the emotion tied
    to those but would not recall the details of events
    that went on in between those events, and they could
    be in an incorrect temporal sequence. In other words,
    you might remember a conversation with Mr. Smith first
    and Mr. Jones second. In fact, that conversation
    could have occurred in reverse order. You may
    remember a conversation with a group of five people on
    28
    
    Id. at 448-50.
    29
    
    Id. at 451.
                                        8
    a particular topic. You then have another
    conversation with seven other people on a different
    topic. You might, when you recall this, incorrectly
    mix up who was involved in which of those
    conversations and incorrectly think that somebody from
    the second conversation actually was also in on the
    first conversation. So you’re capturing the most
    salient, most significant aspects and not capturing
    the things that go in between.
    An en bloc blackout or period of amnesia, similarly,
    would be a consistent period of time. You wouldn’t be
    capturing those moments of salient information. You
    would have a point at which your memory would
    gradually fade out and then you would have a period at
    which you can remember again. Commonly with en bloc
    blackouts the memory ends sometime while you’re still
    awake and active in what you’re doing and doesn’t come
    back until you’ve gone to sleep and wake up the next
    day. It’s less common that someone will have an en
    bloc blackout say from 2200 to 0200 and then all of a
    sudden remember everything clearly from 0200 through
    the rest of the night. Typically the en bloc, the
    very solid blackouts, would last until you’ve gone to
    sleep and your alcohol level has come back down while
    you’re sleeping.
    DC    Okay. Do you believe that—do you agree with Dr. Henry
    that a person only knows if they were in a blackout
    retrospectively, looking back?
    WIT   Yes, you could end up in a different place, you could
    end up with a person you don’t know and have no
    recollection of how that occurred. So you might know
    on your own that you had a blackout or you might be
    involved in a conversation with someone who was at the
    same event that you were at at a later point in time
    and they might ask you about something you said or
    did. You would simply not remember that you said or
    did it. The important thing with blackouts is that
    they—once—you haven’t put it into long-term memory,
    it’s no longer retrievable. It’s not—it’s not that—
    those neurologic changes which put it into long-term
    memory have not occurred. So, if it’s not present the
    next day, it won’t somehow be formed later on because
    the short-term memory is lost.
    MJ    Could you say that again for me, Doctor?
    9
    WIT   The--the process of remembering something is that it’s
    transitioned from short-term memory to long-term
    memory. If that process never occurs in reasonable
    proximity to when the events occurred, it never will
    occur, because the short-term memory will be gone.
    There is no mechanism by which it can move into long-
    term memory after the fact.
    MJ    So the transfer will not have occurred? So there’s no
    memory to remember?
    WIT   Exactly. You haven’t saved the document in your word
    processor. So, when you shut the computer off, it’s
    gone.
    DC    So, with regard to a fragmentary blackout, that
    meaning that some--some memories are retained and some
    memories are just never transmitted into long-term
    memory, for those memories that--that were formed,
    when will those be retrievable?
    WIT   They would be retrievable at the end of that evening,
    for example and be retrievable the next day. As with
    all memories, they would decay with time. So, to the
    extent that they are retrievable, they would be most
    retrievable the next day, the next period of being
    awake and not intoxicated.
    DC    So those fragments that a person is able to remember
    from a fragmentary blackout, is it your testimony that
    if those memories that were truly formed during--
    during the blackout, that they would be retrievable
    the following--the following day?
    WIT   Yes, they would. You might not spontaneously recall
    all of the details. Someone might prompt you and say,
    “Do you also remember that we talked about this?”
    With that prompting you say, “Okay, yes, I remember
    that conversation.” It wasn’t as salient as the other
    pieces of the conversation. So you didn’t—you didn’t
    just spontaneously come up with it, but with a little
    bit of prompting you can expand a bit. There has to
    be the primary memory there to being with if you’re
    going to anything more. Then the prompting would have
    to be very specific to what the primary memory is to
    be able to capture it accurately.
    DC    So, for a memory that was actually formed during a
    fragmentary blackout, if that was not retrieved on the
    first day or the second day but retrieved on the third
    10
    day, based on your understanding of how memory works,
    is that--is that scientifically possible to retrieve
    on a third day a memory that was actually formed
    during a fragmentary blackout?
    WIT   It doesn’t fit with any construct of the way memories
    actually work.
    DC    And why is that, sir?
    WIT   Because you have to go through that process while the
    information is fresh in your mind. It doesn’t--it
    doesn’t sit in a--you know, in a vacuum someplace that
    you can then reopen it later and push it into long-
    term memory. It’s there while it’s there. When the
    period of time has passed, it’s no longer accessible. 30
    The Government recalled Dr. Henry in rebuttal and he
    provided the following testimony:
    TC    Doctor, would you agree with Dr. Grieger’s testimony
    that if you cannot—after—after a night of heavy
    drinking of alcohol, if one cannot recall a memory the
    following day, that they never will?
    WIT   Well, I would respectfully disagree with Dr. Grieger
    on that point. I have never ever heard that. I’ve
    never read that. I think that that may be true of an
    en bloc blackout but certainly is not true of a
    fragmentary blackout. That is not true because I
    don’t see it clinically and, secondly, the science is
    that, in fact, alcohol disrupts that transfer process.
    The transfer process is disorganized. It would then
    stand to reason that, given the disorganization of the
    transfer and encoding process, that later retrieving
    those memories will also be haphazard.
    . . . .
    TC    Doctor, to clarify, would you agree that in an en bloc
    blackout an individual never lays down long-term
    memories during that blackout?
    WIT   In an en bloc, yes, I think I would agree with that
    statement more than I would disagree with it. What I
    would say is when an en bloc blackout occurs there has
    been a disruption in the transfer of memory from
    30
    
    Id. at 541-45.
    11
    short-term to long-term.   It, in a sense, never got
    there.
    TC    Would you agree that in a fragmentary blackout—could
    you describe in a fragmentary blackout, how that is
    different.
    WIT   In a fragmentary blackout the transfer of information
    and the laying down of information occurs but in a
    disorganized and haphazard fashion. That is very
    simple. It--on a neurocellular level, it’s far more
    complex, but for the purposes of this discussion, it
    was laid down and transferred in a disorganized
    fashion. Because of that disorganization in how it
    was laid down, it then cannot oftentimes be retrieved
    in an organized fashion. The manner in which a person
    retrieves fragmentary blackouts is random, just as
    [SW] described. She described pieces. She described
    scenes. She specifically told me there was no order.
    MJ    Doctor, state that again so I can follow you.
    WIT   Sure. In this case [SW] was sleep deprived. For
    example, she said that Monday night she got to the
    hospital at around 10:30 and then didn’t leave until
    5:30 and then went to--directly to her employer’s
    house. She is sleep deprived. Monday night she was
    able to get more sleep. The way the body works is, if
    you are sleep deprived, it will take--it will use an
    opportunity to catch up on sleep. It this case, it
    would make perfect sense that she was then able to
    retrieve her memories several days down the road when
    she caught up in her--in her sleep. The memories that
    she retrieved were fragmented, were disorganized
    because that’s how they were laid down. 31
    Factual Sufficiency
    We review issues of factual sufficiency de novo. United
    States v. Beatty, 
    64 M.J. 456
    , 459 (C.A.A.F. 2007).
    The test for factual sufficiency is “whether, after
    weighing the evidence in the record of trial and making
    allowances for not having personally observed the witnesses, [we
    are] convinced of the [appellant]’s guilt beyond a reasonable
    doubt.” United States v. Turner, 
    25 M.J. 324
    , 325 (C.M.A. 1987).
    In conducting this unique appellate role, we take “a fresh,
    31
    
    Id. at 605-07.
                                       12
    impartial look at the evidence,” applying “neither a presumption
    of innocence nor a presumption of guilt” to “make [our] own
    independent determination as to whether the evidence constitutes
    proof of each required element beyond a reasonable doubt.”
    United States v. Washington, 
    57 M.J. 394
    , 399 (C.A.A.F. 2002).
    Our factual sufficiency determination is limited to a review of
    the “entire record,” meaning evidence presented at trial. United
    States v. Bethea, 
    46 C.M.R. 223
    , 225 (C.M.A. 1973).
    We have reviewed the record of trial and evaluated the
    arguments by the appellant and the Government. Additionally, we
    have made allowances for not having heard and observed the
    witnesses. Having done so, and having considered the unique
    facts of this case, we are not personally convinced of the
    appellant’s guilt of rape or forcible sodomy.
    The appellant was convicted of causing SW to engage in
    sexual intercourse “by using strength, sufficient that she could
    not avoid or escape the sexual contact” and committing sodomy
    with SW “by force and without [her] consent.” The appellant was
    charged under the version of Article 120, UCMJ, in effect from 1
    October 2007 to 27 June 2012, which made it an offense to cause
    another person to engage in a sexual act by using force against
    that other person. Art. 120(a), UCMJ (2007). In pertinent
    part, “force” was defined as, “action to compel submission of
    another or to overcome or prevent another's resistance by . . .
    strength, power, or restraint applied to another person,
    sufficient that the other person could not avoid or escape the
    sexual conduct.” Art.120(t)(5), UCMJ (2007). Similarly, under
    Article 125, UCMJ, “force” is physical violence or power applied
    by the accused to the victim. An act of sodomy occurs “by
    force” when the accused uses physical violence or power to
    compel the victim to submit against his/her will. See Military
    Judges’ Benchbook, Dept. of the Army Pamphlet 27-9 at ¶ 3-51-2
    Note 4 (25 Jun 2014).
    We are unconvinced by the record before us that the
    Government met their heavy burden of proving the required
    element of force for either offense. While SW's description of
    appellant holding her by her arms provided some evidence of
    force, she could not link this action by the appellant to any
    further act, sexual or otherwise, and the disorganized,
    potentially non-sequential order of her memories prevents us
    from concluding that the charged forcible sexual acts
    necessarily followed.
    13
    SW’s segmented memories lacked significant details and she
    could provide no chronology of the events she did remember. The
    events of SW’s segmented memories accounted for at most minutes
    or perhaps only seconds out of at least a seven-hour period and
    there is no further evidence in the record of what happened
    between the appellant and SW during the relevant time frame. As
    our sister court succinctly stated in a recent opinion, it is
    simply not our role to speculate as to what may have occurred
    between the appellant and SW or to fill in the gaps left by the
    Government’s presentation of its case. See United States v.
    Soto, 2014 CCA LEXIS 681, unpublished op. (A.F.Ct.Crim.App. (16
    Sep 2014) (en banc), aff’d, __ M.J. __, 2015 CAAF LEXIS 398
    (C.A.A.F. 2015). The Government’s case rested nearly
    exclusively on SW’s delayed and partial memories that ultimately
    lack the detail and completeness necessary to prove the charges.
    The lack of physical findings to support SW’s description
    of events also gives rise to reasonable doubt. SW testified to
    memories of the appellant holding her down by the arms, using
    his hand to open her mouth, biting her breasts, and taking her
    by the hips and turning her over to her front from her back.
    The sexual assault exam performed within forty-eight hours of
    the incident documented no physical findings on SW’s arms,
    breasts, face, mouth or hips. Nor was there DNA evidence
    introduced linking the appellant and SW in any fashion. The
    primary physical findings were bruises on SW’s legs. Although
    SW was insistent that the bruises did not come from her
    interactions with AM and WC that night, she specifically
    testified that she could recall no actions by the appellant that
    caused the bruises. While we are not suggesting that physical
    findings are required to prove rape or forcible sodomy charges,
    in the case at bar, the lack of such evidence further amplifies
    deficiencies in the Government’s case.
    Finally, the conflicting expert testimony concerning the
    circumstances and validity of SW’s delayed recollection of the
    events at issue contributes to our reasonable doubt in this
    case. Dr. Henry and Dr. Grieger are both qualified experts with
    extensive experience in clinical and forensic psychiatry. Their
    testimony was largely consistent with the notable exception of
    their differing opinions on the reliability of SW’s delayed
    recollection of events. Dr. Grieger stated that SW’s testimony
    regarding her recollections “doesn’t fit with any construct of
    the way memories actually work” and to the extent SW had
    retrievable memories they would be most retrievable the next
    period of being awake and not intoxicated, which in this case
    was Sunday. Dr. Henry disagreed stating that “I have never ever
    14
    heard that. I’ve never read that ... [t]hat is not true because
    I don’t see it clinically and, secondly, the science is that, in
    fact, alcohol disrupts that transfer process.” We find nothing
    in the record to favor one expert’s opinion over the other on
    this point, but we do note that Dr. Henry stressed the
    importance of sleep in the memory recovery process following an
    alcohol induced blackout. We further note SW’s testimony that
    she regained the bulk of her memories throughout the day on
    Tuesday, after she underwent the sexual assault exam at the
    hospital from Monday night until early Tuesday morning.
    Although SW did not testify to how much, if any, sleep she got
    that night, based on the information before us it is reasonable
    to conclude that she did not experience a restful night of sleep
    prior to regaining her memories of the event in question.
    Additionally, contrary to her in court testimony, SW told Dr.
    Henry that she regained her memories of the event over the
    course of a week which helped inform his opinion that SW
    provided him a description that was clinically consistent with
    experiencing a fragmentary blackout.
    Conclusion
    Under the facts presented and for the reasons stated we
    simply are not convinced that the Government satisfied its
    burden of proving the appellant’s guilt to the charges of rape
    and forcible sodomy beyond a reasonable doubt. We therefore
    find the appellant’s convictions factually insufficient. The
    findings of guilty and the sentence are set aside. The charges
    and specifications are dismissed with prejudice.
    For the Court
    R.H. TROIDL
    Clerk of Court
    15
    

Document Info

Docket Number: 201400232

Filed Date: 7/14/2015

Precedential Status: Precedential

Modified Date: 7/16/2015