State v. Haugen ( 2017 )


Menu:
  • 284	                      March 30, 2017	                      No. 19
    IN THE SUPREME COURT OF THE
    STATE OF OREGON
    STATE OF OREGON,
    Respondent on Review,
    v.
    SHAWN EDWIN HAUGEN,
    Petitioner on Review.
    (CC 10CR0636; CA A151535; SC S063754)
    En Banc
    On review from the Court of Appeals.*
    Argued and submitted September 22, 2016.
    Neil F. Byl, Deputy Public Defender, Salem, argued the
    cause and filed the brief for the petitioner on review. Also on
    the briefs was Ernest G. Lannet, Chief Defender, Office of
    Public Defense Services.
    Andrew M. Lavin, Assistant Attorney General, Salem,
    argued the cause and filed the brief for the respondent on
    review. Also on the brief were Ellen F. Rosenblum, Attorney
    General, and Benjamin Gutman, Solicitor General.
    BALDWIN, J.
    The decision of the Court of Appeals is reversed, and the
    case is remanded to the circuit court for further proceedings
    consistent with this decision.
    ______________
    * Appeal from Josephine County Circuit Court, Pat Wolke, Judge. 274 Or
    App 127, 360 P3d 560 (2015).
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	285
    Case Summary: The victim was assaulted late in the evening in the park-
    ing lot of a bar. Immediately after the attack, he could not describe to a police
    officer exactly what had happened or who had assaulted him, but, a few days
    later, the victim identified defendant as one of the perpetrators. Before his trial
    on an assault charge, defendant moved to exclude the eyewitness identification,
    and, applying the then-applicable test for the admissibility of eyewitness testi-
    mony, the trial court ruled that the eyewitness identification was admissible.
    Defendant was convicted of the charged offense. Defendant appealed his convic-
    tion, and while the appeal was pending, the Oregon Supreme Court announced
    its decision in State v. Lawson/James, 352 Or 724, 291 P3d 673 (2012), in which it
    adopted a new test for the admissibility of eyewitness identification evidence. The
    Court of Appeals applied the new test and affirmed defendant’s conviction. Held:
    The identification procedures used in the case raised serious questions about the
    reliability of the victim’s identifications of defendant under Lawson/James, and,
    therefore, remand is necessary to give the trial court the opportunity to consider
    the admissibility of the identifications under the correct standard.
    The decision of the Court of Appeals is reversed, and the case is remanded to
    the trial court for further proceedings consistent with this decision.
    286	                                         State v. Haugen
    BALDWIN, J.
    In this criminal case, defendant was convicted
    of one count of third-degree assault, based mainly on the
    victim’s eyewitness identification of him. Before his trial,
    defendant moved to exclude the eyewitness identification.
    Applying the test for admissibility of eyewitness testimony
    set out in State v. Classen, 285 Or 221, 590 P2d 1198 (1979),
    the trial court ruled that the victim’s eyewitness identifica-
    tion was admissible. While the case was pending on appeal,
    this court announced its decision in State v. Lawson/James,
    352 Or 724, 291 P3d 673 (2012), in which the court substan-
    tially revised the Classen test for determining the admis-
    sibility of eyewitness testimony. In the Court of Appeals,
    defendant argued that the identification procedures used in
    this case raised serious questions about the reliability of the
    identification under Lawson/James, and, therefore, that the
    Court of Appeals should remand the case to the trial court
    for a new hearing and trial, with the trial court utilizing
    the Lawson/James test. The Court of Appeals disagreed,
    concluding that, even under Lawson/James, the trial court
    correctly denied defendant’s motion to suppress. State v.
    Haugen, 274 Or App 127, 360 P3d 560 (2015). We allowed
    review and, for the reasons explained below, we reverse the
    decision of the Court of Appeals and remand the case to
    the trial court for further proceedings consistent with this
    decision.
    BACKGROUND
    The following facts are undisputed. In September
    2010, the victim met two friends at a bar in Grants Pass,
    Oregon. Already present were several people wearing cloth-
    ing indicating that they were members of the Vagos motor-
    cycle gang. The victim did not know any of the Vagos mem-
    bers present at the bar, but one of the victim’s companions
    knew and had a conversation with one of the gang members,
    a man later identified as Rives. During the evening, Rives
    approached the table where the victim was sitting and asked
    the victim if he knew a man named Moore, a former member
    of the Vagos who, several years earlier, had been a witness
    for the prosecution against other Vagos members. The vic-
    tim answered, “Well, yes I do. I understand, you know, he
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	287
    used to be a Vago.” Rives then became irate and began rant-
    ing about Moore. The victim merely nodded in response. The
    encounter lasted two to three minutes.
    The victim was at the bar for about two hours.
    During that time, he overheard a conversation in which
    several gang members were teasing defendant, who also
    appeared to be a member of the Vagos, about wearing a red
    shirt and tie to sell cars on a car lot. When the victim was
    leaving the bar around 12:30 a.m., defendant, who was then
    standing in a hall near the exit door, looked directly at the
    victim and said, “Have a good fucking night.” As the victim
    stepped outside the door to the bar, he encountered Rives.
    The victim thought that he had heard something behind
    him, turned to look, and saw someone whom he could not
    clearly see holding the door shut. Rives then said to the vic-
    tim, “Are you here to kick us out?” The victim responded,
    “No, I’m going home. I just want to walk to my car.” At that
    moment, the victim saw that defendant was standing to his
    right. Defendant touched the victim’s shoulder and said,
    “Well, walk to your car.” As the victim began walking to his
    car, someone punched him on the side of the head. He fell to
    the ground and someone kicked him in the chest or shoul-
    der. As the victim tried to get up, someone else struck him
    in the head with what he later thought was a small metal
    hammer. The assailants then left. The victim was nearly
    knocked unconscious but was able to get up and walk to his
    truck and drive home.
    Once home, the victim called 9-1-1 and reported
    that he had been assaulted by several members of the Vagos
    motorcycle gang and that he had been punched and “blind-
    sided.” A police officer, Nicklason, arrived later that night
    to interview the victim. The victim appeared to be in pain
    and to have been significantly injured.1 The victim told
    Nicklason that four to six Vagos members approached him
    as he was leaving the bar and asked him if he was trying to
    run them off. The victim stated that he was later struck on
    the right side by someone he did not see. He thought he had
    been punched but was not sure. He did not mention being hit
    1
    As the victim later testified, he was “really dazed and * * * really out of it,”
    and he “was in intense pain” when he was talking to Nicklason.
    288	                                                      State v. Haugen
    with a hammer.2 The victim told Nicklason that there were
    no witnesses to the assault, and, although he recognized his
    assailants as part of a group that had been in the bar, “he
    couldn’t recognize them specifically individually.” That is,
    the victim did not describe to Nicklason the race, height,
    weight, or any identifying feature of the individuals who
    assaulted him, other than that they were male and mem-
    bers of the Vagos gang. The victim attributed his inability
    to provide a description of his assailants to the facts that it
    was dark and it was a brief encounter.
    Nicklason forwarded the case to Detective Brown,
    who was responsible for dealing with outlaw motorcycle
    gangs in the area. Brown interviewed the victim at the
    police station five days later. The victim once again relayed
    the events of the evening when he was assaulted, this time
    in more detail. The victim told Brown that the people at the
    bar were definitely members of the Vagos gang—they were
    “flying their colors,” that is, wearing green bandanas and
    jackets with the Vagos insignia. The victim stated that the
    person who had struck him from the side was a “great big
    guy,” about 230 pounds, not fat, in his late 20s or early 30s,
    and had been teased at the bar earlier for dressing up for his
    job as a car salesman. The victim also told Brown that the
    person who had hit him with the hammer was a “little fat
    guy,” probably in his 40s, with a long ponytail. Brown asked
    the victim if the first assailant was “pretty buff.” When the
    victim answered in the affirmative, Brown said, “I think I
    know who you’re talking about.” After talking to the victim
    for a few more minutes, Brown said, “[W]hat we’ll do here
    in a minute, [victim], I’ll show you some photographs—and
    maybe that will help us—once we have some photos we can
    go through—and we’ll identify—who the little fat guy is.”
    Brown then gave the victim the following disclaimer:
    “You are about to be shown some photographs. Just
    because the officer is showing you these photos, you are
    in no way obligated to identify anybody, okay? The person
    2
    As the victim confirmed at trial, on the night of the assault, he did not
    recall being hit with a hammer. When asked at trial whether, that night, he
    remembered a ball-peen hammer being used, the victim testified, “No, I didn’t. I
    was really out of it.”
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	289
    who committed the crime may or may not be in this group
    of photographs.”
    Brown explained to the victim that he would ordinarily show
    him six photographs, “but because there’s so many people
    involved in this I’m just going to show you a group of photo-
    graphs.” Brown then presented the victim with a binder of
    23 photos. The photos were not chosen based on the subjects’
    similarities to the descriptions that the victim provided to
    Brown of his assailants and the other people in their group;
    rather, all were Department of Motor Vehicle driver license
    photographs of known Vagos “Outlaw Motorcycle Gang”
    members and associates. The photos did not include any
    notes or names.
    The victim identified one person he recognized as
    a “good man,” and Brown agreed and provided that indi-
    vidual’s name. Next, the victim identified a man he knew
    as “Ronnie.” Brown confirmed that man’s name. The vic-
    tim thought that Ronnie had been present, but he was not
    sure whether Ronnie was involved in the assault. Then
    the victim pointed to a picture of defendant, stating, “That
    sure looks like the guy that hit me right there,” and “I’m
    pretty sure it was him.” Brown said, “We’ll just call him
    White Boy for now,” and set that picture aside. After iden-
    tifying another person he thought was present at the bar,
    the victim asked Brown how old the pictures were. Brown
    responded that they were a couple of years old. Brown and
    the victim continued through the remaining pictures, with
    the victim pointing out people he thought might have been
    present in the bar on the night of the assault as well as
    people he knew, and with Brown providing feedback such
    as providing the real names and gang names of some of the
    people pictured and commenting on who was in prison, who
    had distinctive physical traits or habits, whose hair had or
    had not changed since the DMV photo was taken, and the
    like.
    At one point, Brown directed the victim’s attention
    to a picture of Rives, saying, “This gentleman now has a lit-
    tle bit more gray. I’m talking about ‘Six Ball,’ or Steve Rives.
    He’s got more gray, but he’s got a ponytail that long on his
    back.” The victim responded,
    290	                                             State v. Haugen
    “That sure looks like the little fat man right there. He looks
    different in this. His hair appears darker. But that sure
    looks like him because he did—his hair was lighter and the
    ponytail was probably about that long. * * * I believe he’s
    one of the ones involved. I wanted to come back to him, but
    * * * that’s why I asked you when I looked at his picture how
    old these pictures are.”
    Brown set that photo aside as well.
    Brown and the victim examined the remaining pic-
    tures, with Brown continuing his commentary and the vic-
    tim pointing out individuals whom he thought might have
    been present at the bar on the night of the assault. When
    they had finished going through the photos, Brown returned
    the victim’s attention to defendant’s picture. Brown asked
    the victim how certain he was that defendant was the per-
    son who first struck him. The victim replied that he was 75
    to 80 percent certain. He also told Brown that defendant
    was the person who, earlier in the night, had said to him,
    “Have a good fucking night.”
    Brown then offered to present the victim with a
    series of photos of defendant and others that had been taken
    during an unrelated surveillance action a week earlier, say-
    ing that “maybe that will help a little bit.” Brown told the
    victim that he wanted to show him the more recent photos
    “because there are some significant differences in both the
    gentlemen that you have said that have assaulted you.” In
    the second set of photos, the pictured individuals were all
    wearing gang apparel. The victim identified Rives as the
    person who had hit him with the hammer, saying that he
    was “99.9 percent sure” it was Rives. The victim also iden-
    tified defendant in one of the photos and again said that he
    was 80 percent certain that defendant was the person who
    first struck him. After being shown yet another recent sur-
    veillance photo of a group of individuals that included defen-
    dant, the victim raised his certainty level to 90 percent.
    Defendant was charged with one count of third-
    degree assault. Before the trial, defendant moved to suppress
    the victim’s identification of him during the interview with
    Brown and to suppress any future in-court identification.
    Defendant argued that the identification was inadmissible
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	291
    under Classen, because the lineup procedure Brown had
    used was suggestive, it needlessly departed from prescribed
    procedures for avoiding suggestiveness, and the circum-
    stances surrounding the identification indicated that the
    victim’s identification was not made independent of the sug-
    gestive lineup. The trial court denied the motion. In a let-
    ter opinion, the trial court ruled that the lineup procedure
    that Brown had used was not unduly suggestive, because
    Brown did not violate guidelines for photographic identifica-
    tions and because the court could find no other legal reason
    for concluding that the photo lineup was unduly suggestive.
    The trial court also found that the victim’s identification
    was reliable when considered under four of the factors that
    this court had set out for determining eyewitness identifi-
    cation reliability in Classen (the witness’s ability to observe
    the defendant; the timing and completeness of the witness’s
    description; the witness’s certainty in the identification; and
    the lapse of time between the event and the identification)
    and in light of the fact that various details that the victim
    remembered about his assailant were later corroborated by
    other witnesses at the bar. For those reasons, the trial court
    concluded that “the accuracy of the identification in this
    case is a matter for the trial jury; and not for this court.”
    The case proceeded to trial in April 2012. During
    the trial, the victim identified defendant in court as the per-
    son who had punched and kicked him, as the person who
    had said, as the victim was leaving the bar, “Have a good
    fucking night,” and as the person who had been teased as
    a car salesman at the bar. The victim testified that he was
    100 percent certain that defendant was the person who
    assaulted him. Ultimately, the jury convicted defendant of
    the charged offense.
    After defendant filed the notice of appeal in this
    case, this court issued its opinion in Lawson/James, which
    we discuss in more detail below. Defendant argued to the
    Court of Appeals that Lawson/James dramatically changed
    the legal landscape for determining whether an eyewitness
    identification is admissible and that serious questions about
    the reliability of the victim’s identification of defendant under
    Lawson/James warranted reversal and remand for a new
    292	                                           State v. Haugen
    hearing in which the trial court could apply the Lawson/
    James test. As noted, the Court of Appeals concluded that,
    even under Lawson/James, the trial court correctly denied
    defendant’s motion to suppress.
    ANALYSIS
    As discussed, the trial court applied the Classen
    test at the suppression hearing to determine the reliabil-
    ity, and thus the admissibility, of the eyewitness identifica-
    tion. It is helpful at this point, then, to describe the Classen
    methodology, which was intended to ensure that only suf-
    ficiently reliable identifications are admitted into evidence.
    Classen required a two-step inquiry: First, the court was
    to determine whether the process leading to the identifica-
    tion was suggestive or needlessly departed from procedures
    prescribed to avoid suggestiveness. 285 Or at 232. If so,
    then, second, the court was to determine whether the pros-
    ecution had established that there was some source for the
    identification other than the suggestive procedure or that
    other evidence substantially negated the risk that the iden-
    tification was stimulated by the suggestive procedures. 
    Id. Classen listed
    five nonexclusive factors for the trial court to
    consider in determining whether an identification had been
    made independent of the suggestive procedure: (1) the wit-
    ness’s opportunity to clearly view the suspect; (2) the atten-
    tion that the witness had paid to the suspect’s features;
    (3) the timing and completeness of the witness’s description
    of the suspect; (4) the witness’s certainty in his or her identi-
    fication; and (5) the lapse of time between the original obser-
    vation and the identification. 
    Id. at 232-33.
    In this case, the
    trial court found that the identification process that Brown
    had used was not suggestive. That is, the court found that
    Brown did not needlessly depart from police guidelines
    for conducting identifications and that there was no other
    legal basis for concluding that the process was suggestive.
    Notwithstanding that initial conclusion, the trial court also
    reviewed the Classen factors and concluded that the victim’s
    identification of defendant was made independent of the
    identification procedure used.
    While the case was pending on appeal, this
    court decided Lawson/James, which, as we have noted,
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	293
    significantly changed the framework for determining the
    admissibility of eyewitness identification testimony that is
    asserted to have been tainted by suggestive police practices.
    The impetus for revisiting the analysis that the court had
    used in Classen was new scientific research surrounding
    eyewitness identifications. Lawson/James, 352 Or at 739-
    40. In particular, the court discussed two categories of fac-
    tors that, according to the scientific community, affect the
    reliability of eyewitness identification: so-called “estimator
    variables” and “system variables.” Estimator variables are
    “characteristics of the witness, the alleged perpetrator, and
    the environmental conditions of the event that cannot be
    manipulated or adjusted by state actors.” 
    Id. at 740.3
    System
    variables relate “to the circumstances surrounding the iden-
    tification procedure itself that are generally within the con-
    trol of those administering the procedure.” Id.4 In light of the
    scientific research and those variables, the court in Lawson/
    James concluded that the process outlined in Classen did
    “not accomplish its goal of ensuring that only sufficiently
    reliable identifications are admitted into evidence.” 
    Id. at 746.
    In fact, the court stated, the reliability factors that
    the court listed in Classen were both incomplete and, at
    times, inconsistent with modern scientific findings,5 and the
    Classen inquiry itself was “somewhat at odds with its own
    goals and with current Oregon evidence law.” 
    Id. Moreover, 3
           Estimator variables include, among other things, whether the witness was
    under stress at the time of the incident; where the witness’s attention was focused
    during the incident; the amount of time that the witness spent looking at the per-
    petrator; the environmental viewing conditions; factors affecting the witness’s
    visual, physical, and mental acuity; distinctive characteristics of the perpetrator;
    and the time elapsed between the incident and the identification. 
    Id. at 744-46.
    	4
    System variables include, among other things, whether the identification
    was conducted by a “blind” administrator (a person who does not know the iden-
    tity of the suspect); whether the witness was instructed prior to the procedure
    that the suspect may or may not be among those shown; whether the lineup was
    made up of subjects based on their physical similarity to the witness’s descrip-
    tion of the perpetrator; whether the individuals in the lineup or photo array are
    shown sequentially or simultaneously; whether the witness was shown the sus-
    pect multiple times throughout the course of the investigation; whether there
    was evidence of suggestive questioning or other sources of post-event memory
    contamination; and whether the administrator provided suggestive or confirm-
    ing feedback. 
    Id. at 741-44.
    	5
    To take one example, research shows that a witness’s confidence or level of
    certainty (one of the Classen factors) may not be a good indicator of identification
    accuracy, but it nonetheless has a substantial potential to influence jurors. 
    Id. at 745.
    294	                                                       State v. Haugen
    the Classen test prohibited courts from considering whether
    an identification was reliable until evidence of some sugges-
    tiveness was introduced. 
    Id. at 746.
    And the Classen factors
    relied heavily on the witness’s self-reports to establish the
    existence or nonexistence of suggestibility factors, when
    current scientific research indicates that self-reported evi-
    dence of reliability can be inflated or exaggerated by the
    suggestive procedure itself. 
    Id. at 748.
    	         The court in Lawson/James therefore set out a new
    framework for determining the reliability of eyewitness
    identification testimony, based on generally applicable pro-
    visions of the Oregon Evidence Code, including the founda-
    tional principles that, under OEC 402, all relevant evidence
    is admissible; that, under OEC 401, evidence is relevant if
    it has any tendency to make the existence of a fact of conse-
    quence to the determination of the action more or less prob-
    able than it would be without the evidence; and that, under
    OEC 403, relevant evidence may be excluded if its probative
    value is substantially outweighed by the danger of unfair
    prejudice. Lawson/James, 352 Or at 752, 756. The court
    announced a two-step process for determining the admissi-
    bility of eyewitness identification testimony. First,
    “[w]hen a witness’s perceptions are capable of supporting
    an inference of identification, but are nevertheless met
    with competing evidence of an impermissible basis for that
    inference—i.e., suggestive police procedures—an issue of
    fact arises as to whether the witness’s subsequent identi-
    fication was derived from a permissible or impermissible
    basis.”
    
    Id. at 755.
    At that point, the state, as the proponent of the
    evidence, has the burden to present sufficient evidence from
    which a juror could find that the witness’s identification was
    derived from a permissible basis. State v. Hickman, 355 Or
    715, 729, 330 P3d 551 (2014).6 That is, to meet its prelim-
    inary burden to demonstrate that eyewitness testimony
    6
    In Lawson/James, the court described the state’s burden at this point as
    establishing by a “preponderance of the evidence” that the identification was
    based on a permissible basis. 352 Or at 755. In Hickman, the court clarified that,
    under OEC 602, a preponderance of the evidence need only present evidence from
    which a reasonable juror could find that the eyewitness observed facts necessary
    to make an identification. 355 Or at 728-30.
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	295
    is relevant under OEC 402, the state must provide “proof
    under OEC 602 that the proffered eyewitness has personal
    knowledge of the matters to which the witness will testify,
    and proof under OEC 701 that any identification is both
    rationally based on the witness’s first-hand perceptions and
    helpful to the trier of fact.” Lawson/James, 352 Or at 761-62.
    As the court stated,
    “Although the initial admissibility requirements for eye-
    witness identification evidence establish a minimum base-
    line of reliability, the persuasive power of the evidence that
    meets that standard may nevertheless vary greatly, and
    many identifications possessing relatively low probative
    value may still pass that initial test.”
    
    Id. at 758.
    	        If the state introduces evidence sufficient to sup-
    port a finding that the eyewitness testimony is not barred
    by OEC 402, then the second step comes into play. At that
    point, the defendant assumes the burden of proving that
    OEC 403 nevertheless requires its exclusion. 
    Id. at 757.
    The
    court stated,
    “Thus, even after finding that the evidence meets the min-
    imum requirements of OEC 602 and 701, trial courts must
    still conduct a thorough examination of all the pertinent
    factors in order to determine the probative value of the evi-
    dence under OEC 403.”
    
    Id. at 758.
    	        Probative value is determined under OEC 403 by
    considering the relative reliability of the eyewitness testi-
    mony, given the system variables and the estimator vari-
    ables. The court stated, “The more factors—the presence
    of system variables alone or in combination with estimator
    variables—that weigh against reliability of the identifica-
    tion, the less persuasive the identification evidence will be
    to prove the fact of identification, and correspondingly, the
    less probative value that identification will have.” 
    Id. at 757.
    Further, the court stated,
    “As a discrete evidentiary class, eyewitness identifications
    subjected to suggestive police procedures are particularly
    susceptible to concerns of unfair prejudice. Consequently,
    296	                                           State v. Haugen
    in cases in which an eyewitness has been exposed to sug-
    gestive police procedures, trial courts have a heightened
    role as an evidentiary gatekeeper because ‘traditional’
    methods of testing reliability—like cross-examination—
    can be ineffective at discrediting unreliable or inaccurate
    eyewitness identification evidence.”
    
    Id. at 758.
    	         Moreover, the court stated, expert testimony is par-
    ticularly useful to this inquiry for two reasons. First, “tra-
    ditional methods of informing factfinders of the pitfalls of
    eyewitness identification—cross-examination, closing argu-
    ment, and generalized jury instructions—frequently are not
    adequate to inform factfinders of the factors affecting the
    reliability of such identifications.” 
    Id. at 759.
    And second,
    system and estimator variables may be unknown to fact-
    finders or contrary to common assumptions. 
    Id. at 761.
    	         Ultimately, though, as the court stated, the decision
    whether to admit or exclude eyewitness identification evi-
    dence is committed to the discretion of the trial court, and,
    as the court observed, “it is doubtful that issues concerning
    one or more of the estimator variables that we have identi-
    fied will, without more, be enough to support an inference of
    unreliability sufficient to justify the exclusion of the eyewit-
    ness identification.” 
    Id. at 762;
    see also Hickman, 355 Or at
    726 (same).
    The court then applied its new framework to the
    facts in Lawson and James. In Lawson, the court concluded
    that the record raised serious concerns about the reliability
    of the identification evidence proffered below. In that case,
    the victim and her husband had driven in their trailer to a
    campsite where the victim’s husband earlier had pitched a
    tent. When they arrived, the defendant’s truck was parked
    in their parking space and the defendant had moved into
    their tent. The husband told the defendant that it was their
    tent. The defendant apologized, moved his belongings out
    of the tent and into his truck, and moved to a vacant camp-
    site nearby. The defendant was in the victim’s view for about
    40 minutes. Later that same evening, the victim was stand-
    ing by the window of the trailer when she was shot in the
    chest with a large caliber hunting rifle. Her husband called
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	297
    9-1-1, but he too was shot while speaking with the operator,
    and he died shortly thereafter. The dispatcher called back
    and the victim told the dispatcher that she and her husband
    had been shot and that she did not know who had shot them.
    When first responders arrived, they found the vic-
    tim lying in the trailer, seriously injured but conscious. She
    was transported to a hospital. An ambulance attendant
    testified that the victim was rambling and hysterical, and
    other medical personnel reported that the victim referred
    to the shooter as “they,” mentioned several different people
    as the shooter, and stated that she had not seen the perpe-
    trator and did not know who it was. The victim was near
    death when she arrived at the hospital and was immediately
    taken into surgery.
    When a police detective interviewed the victim the
    next day, she was heavily medicated and sedated, and she
    could not speak because of a breathing tube in her throat.
    The defendant had come to the attention of police officers
    when he had volunteered to them that he had encountered
    the victim and her husband at the campsite in the morn-
    ing on the day that they were shot, and the detective later
    showed the victim a photo lineup that included a picture of
    the defendant. The victim was unable to identify the defen-
    dant in that photo lineup. After the detective posed some
    leading questions, the victim agreed that she had seen the
    person who had shot her earlier in the day at the campsite,
    several hours before she was shot. Two weeks later, when
    interviewed again, the victim remembered that the perpe-
    trator had covered her face with a pillow after entering the
    trailer so as to obscure her view of him, and she said that
    she could not identify the man because of the pillow and
    because it was dark. Two weeks after that, the victim told
    the detective that, notwithstanding the pillow over her face,
    she had briefly seen the perpetrator in the trailer, but she
    would not be able to pick the perpetrator’s picture out of a
    photo lineup. Between then and the trial two years later,
    however, on multiple separate occasions, the victim was
    exposed to the defendant’s likeness in contexts where the
    defendant was clearly the suspect who had been arrested
    for the crime. Ultimately, at the defendant’s trial, the victim
    298	                                         State v. Haugen
    identified the defendant as the man who had shot her and
    her husband.
    The trial court denied the defendant’s motion to
    strike the identification, and the defendant was later con-
    victed of aggravated murder, attempted aggravated murder,
    and other crimes. The Court of Appeals affirmed the defen-
    dant’s convictions, finding that, under Classen, the identifi-
    cation procedures prior to the trial were suggestive, but con-
    cluding, after weighing the Classen factors, that the victim’s
    identification of the defendant had been made independent
    of the suggestive procedures and, therefore, that the trial
    court had not erred in declining to strike the identification.
    State v. Lawson, 239 Or App 363, 244 P3d 860 (2010).
    On review, this court considered its new framework
    for determining the admissibility of eyewitness testimony
    and observed that several estimator variables and system
    variables were at play. First, factors falling into the cate-
    gory of estimator variables, which negatively affected the
    reliability of the identification, included the facts that the
    eyewitness—the victim—was under tremendous stress from
    the shooting of her husband and in poor physical and mental
    condition from being shot herself when she first observed
    the perpetrator entering the trailer; the environmental con-
    ditions were poor, as it was dark and the victim was on the
    floor when she observed the perpetrator; the perpetrator had
    covered her head with a pillow; the victim stated repeatedly
    that she never got a good look at the perpetrator; and the
    in-court identification was over two years after the incident.
    Lawson/James, 352 Or at 763-64.
    Second, factors falling into the category of system
    variables included the fact that the detective first inter-
    viewed the victim when she was heavily medicated and
    sedated, which, the court stated, together with her impaired
    view of the defendant, would have made her especially vul-
    nerable to memory contamination from suggestive question-
    ing. 
    Id. at 764.
    Additionally, the police then questioned her
    using leading questions that implicitly communicated their
    belief that the defendant was the shooter and implanted in
    her mind that the person she had seen earlier in the day at
    the campsite was the perpetrator. 
    Id. According to
    the court,
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	299
    those factors could have affected every subsequent attempt
    that the victim made to recall the perpetrator, and, from
    that point forward, also would have made it extremely diffi-
    cult for the victim to mentally separate her brief glimpse of
    the man in the trailer from the person she had seen at the
    campsite for 40 minutes in broad daylight earlier in the day.
    
    Id. Finally, the
    victim was unable to identify the defendant
    until after she had seen his likeness numerous times in sug-
    gestive circumstances, but, notwithstanding that fact, the
    victim identified the defendant at the trial and testified that
    she had absolutely no doubt as to her identification, stating,
    “I’ll never forget his face as long as I live. * * * I always knew
    it was him.” This court stated that the alteration in the
    victim’s statements over time were indicative of a memory
    altered by suggestion and confirming feedback. 
    Id. at 765.
    	       After discussing the foregoing estimator variables
    and system variables, the court concluded:
    “In light of current scientific knowledge regarding the
    effects of suggestion and confirming feedback, the preced-
    ing circumstances raise serious questions concerning the
    reliability of the identification evidence admitted at defen-
    dant’s trial. In Lawson, because the Court of Appeals and
    trial court relied on the procedures set out in Classen—
    procedures that we have revised in this opinion—we reverse
    and remand the case to the trial court for a new trial. Due
    to the novelty and complexity of the procedures we have
    articulated today, the parties must be permitted on retrial
    to (1) supplement the record with any additional evidence
    that may bear on the reliability of the eyewitness identifi-
    cations at issue here, and (2) present arguments regarding
    the appropriate application of the new procedures set out
    in this opinion.”
    
    Id. Notably, the
    court did not conduct its own inquiry into
    the preliminary question whether the identification was
    barred under OEC 402 because the witness’s perceptions
    were incapable of supporting an inference of identification
    or any balancing of probative value against prejudicial effect
    under OEC 403.
    Turning next to James, the court reached a differ-
    ent conclusion; the court held that application of the revised
    framework “could not have resulted in the exclusion of the
    300	                                         State v. Haugen
    eyewitness identification evidence.” 
    Id. James involved
    a
    midday theft from a grocery store. A store clerk had seen
    two Native American men—one small and one large—
    stuffing 40-ounce bottles of Steel Reserve 211 beer into a
    backpack. He alerted the assistant store manager and
    pointed out to him the two perpetrators as they were leaving
    the store. The employees were unable to stop the men, and
    they drove away in a gray van.
    The employees called the police and, when the
    officer arrived, they gave him a detailed description of the
    perpetrators, including their race, approximate height and
    weight, hair styles, and clothing. In that interview, both
    employees were adamant that they would be able to iden-
    tify the perpetrators if they saw them again. A few hours
    later, the officer saw two men who matched the description
    of the perpetrators of the grocery store theft “exactly”—the
    defendant (the larger man) and another man—and ques-
    tioned them. The suspects denied any involvement in the
    theft but consented to a search of a backpack in their posses-
    sion, which contained an unopened 40-ounce bottle of Steel
    Reserve 211 beer. The officer asked the men if they would be
    willing to go to the grocery store with him to “clear the mat-
    ter up.” The men consented. The officer handcuffed the men
    and drove them to the grocery store, where he presented
    them to the employees for identification. The employees posi-
    tively identified both suspects as the men who had stolen the
    beer.
    The defendant was charged with multiple offenses.
    Before trial, he moved to suppress both the out-of-court
    identification and any in-court identification by the employ-
    ees. He contended that the identification procedure in the
    parking lot was unduly suggestive, and, therefore, the iden-
    tification was inadmissible under Classen. Although the
    trial court agreed that the identification procedure was sug-
    gestive, it denied the motion to suppress on the ground that
    the identification was made independent of the suggestive
    procedures.
    On review, this court considered, as it had in
    Lawson, whether application of the revised test for admissi-
    bility of eyewitness identification testimony could result in a
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	301
    different outcome; in James the court concluded that it could
    not. The court observed that, within minutes of the crime,
    the employees had given a police officer a detailed and accu-
    rate description of the suspects, including information about
    their race, size, weight, clothing, and the backpack that they
    were using. Lawson/James, 352 Or at 765-66. The officer
    apprehended two men a few hours later who matched the
    employees’ descriptions of the perpetrators “exactly,” and
    the employees later confirmed that the men whom the officer
    had apprehended were the men they had seen in the store.
    
    Id. at 766.
    	        The court then analyzed the admissibility of that
    identification under the framework that it had just adopted.
    First, the court concluded that the OEC 602 requirement
    of personal knowledge was met. The court stated that,
    although some facts falling into the category of estimator
    variables could have negatively affected the witnesses’ per-
    ceptions (stress from the assault, for example), on the whole,
    the estimator variables suggested reliability: both employ-
    ees were face-to-face with the perpetrators and had a clear
    opportunity to observe them for a lengthy period of time,
    they saw the perpetrators in good environmental conditions
    (inside a lighted store and outside, in broad daylight), and
    the perpetrators had distinctive features. 
    Id. From that,
    the
    court concluded that “no reasonable decisionmaker could
    find that the witnesses did not have the personal knowledge
    necessary to identify the perpetrators.” 
    Id. Because the
    identification occurred during a “show
    up” procedure that the trial court had found to be unduly
    suggestive, however, the state, as the proponent of the iden-
    tifications, was required under OEC 701 to introduce suffi-
    cient evidence to support a finding that the witnesses’ obser-
    vations were based on their original observations, untainted
    by the suggestive procedures. 
    Id. at 767.
    This court noted
    that the trial court had been satisfied that the suggestive
    show-up confrontation did not contribute to the witnesses
    identification of the defendant, because the employees had
    had a very good look at the perpetrators during the crime
    and had described them with particularity, including pro-
    viding details about their features, the clothing that they
    were wearing, and the unusual-sized bottles of an unusual
    302	                                         State v. Haugen
    brand of beer that they had stolen. 
    Id. As this
    court stated,
    the witnesses’ accuracy in describing those details demon-
    strated their reliability, and, therefore, the court held, the
    trial court did not err in reaching its factual conclusion that
    the witnesses’ identifications of the defendant were based on
    their original observations. 
    Id. The court
    then turned to the final analytical steps
    under the new framework: determining whether the wit-
    nesses’ identifications were helpful to the trier of fact and
    whether OEC 403 required their exclusion. 
    Id. On those
    points, the court acknowledged that an argument could
    be made that the witnesses’ identifications did not provide
    the jury with information that was more helpful than their
    descriptions of the perpetrators, and that, therefore, the
    persuasive value of the identifications was limited and out-
    weighed by the unfair prejudice introduced by the identifi-
    cations. 
    Id. But, the
    court held, the concern of unfair prej-
    udice was negligible. 
    Id. According to
    the court, because
    the employees’ descriptions of the perpetrators so closely
    matched the two men apprehended by the police, their later
    identification of the defendant could have prejudiced him lit-
    tle, if at all. 
    Id. at 767-68.
    	        To summarize, in Lawson, consideration of both
    estimator variables and system variables raised serious
    questions about the reliability of the witness’s identifica-
    tion of the defendant. Because, for that reason, the result
    could have been different under an application of the revised
    framework, the court reversed the defendant’s conviction
    and remanded the case to the trial court for a new suppres-
    sion hearing and trial. In James, the court noted that facts
    falling into the category of system variables rendered the
    identification procedure suggestive, but because the wit-
    nesses had had the opportunity to view the perpetrators for
    a lengthy period of time during the commission of the crime
    and had been able to give the police officer a detailed and
    accurate description of the perpetrators immediately after
    the crimes, the court was able to determine that any error in
    applying the Classen framework was harmless. That is, even
    under the new framework, no reasonable decision-maker
    could have found that the witnesses did not have personal
    knowledge necessary to identify the perpetrators, the state
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	303
    would have been able to introduce sufficient evidence to sup-
    port a finding that the witness identifications were based on
    their original observations, and admission of the identifica-
    tion evidence did not unduly prejudice the defendant.
    APPLICATION OF LAWSON/JAMES
    As we have stated, the trial court, in relying on
    Classen, applied what we now view as an incorrect legal
    standard. On review, we consider whether there was “lit-
    tle likelihood” that application of that incorrect legal stan-
    dard affected the verdict. Hickman, 355 Or at 749 (applying
    standard); State v. Davis, 336 Or 19, 32, 77 P3d 1111 (2003)
    (“Oregon’s constitutional test for affirmance despite error
    consists of a single inquiry: Is there little likelihood that the
    particular error affected the verdict?”). Only if we are able
    to conclude that application of the Lawson/James test could
    not have resulted in the exclusion of the eyewitness identifi-
    cation evidence will we affirm the ruling of the trial court.
    See Lawson/James, 352 Or at 765.
    The Court of Appeals did so conclude and held that
    the trial court correctly denied defendant’s motion to dis-
    miss. Haugen, 274 Or App at 150. In so holding, the Court
    of Appeals first considered whether the record demon-
    strated that the state introduced sufficient evidence to per-
    mit a finding that the eyewitness testimony would not be
    barred under OEC 402. To that end, the court found, first,
    that the victim’s identification satisfied the low threshold of
    relevance under OEC 401. 
    Id. at 146.
    Second, the Court of
    Appeals concluded, based on the facts that the victim was
    able to describe defendant in detail at the police station
    several days after the attack and that the victim remem-
    bered both defendant and Rives talking to him immedi-
    ately before the assault, that there was “more than enough
    [evidence] to demonstrate, for purposes of OEC 602, that
    the victim’s identification was based on personal knowl-
    edge.” 
    Id. at 146.
    Third, the Court of Appeals rejected
    defendant’s challenges to the identification procedures that
    Brown had used and concluded that the question whether
    there was sufficient evidence to permit a finding that the
    victim’s identification was based on his first-hand percep-
    tions so as to satisfy OEC 701 could reasonably be resolved
    304	                                                        State v. Haugen
    only in the state’s favor. 
    Id. at 147.
    In light of those conclu-
    sions, the court held, “the victim’s identifications of defen-
    dant meet the threshold requirements for admissibility
    under Lawson/James.” 
    Id. at 150.
    The Court of Appeals
    then stated that, therefore,
    “defendant can prevail only by showing that the evidence is
    inadmissible under OEC 403 because its probative value is
    outweighed by the danger of unfair prejudice.”
    
    Id. And, the
    court stated, because defendant had made no
    OEC 403 argument on appeal, OEC 403 provided no basis
    for excluding the evidence. In light of that holding, the Court
    of Appeals did not consider whether any of the estimator
    variables or system variables presented in the case raised
    serious questions about the reliability, and thus the proba-
    tive value, of the victim’s identifications.
    While we agree with the Court of Appeals that the
    state met its burden under the Lawson/James test to estab-
    lish a “minimum baseline of reliability,”7 Lawson/James, 352
    Or at 758, we disagree with the Court of Appeals’ conclusion
    that OEC 403 provided no basis for excluding the identifi-
    cations. Although, in his argument to the Court of Appeals,
    defendant did not cite OEC 403 as the specific basis for
    remanding the case to the trial court for a new suppression
    hearing, we cannot agree that defendant failed to preserve
    an OEC 403 argument on appeal. In the Court of Appeals,
    defendant asserted that, in Lawson/James, this court had
    replaced the Classen test for determining the admissibility
    of eyewitness identification evidence with a new test that
    requires consideration of system and estimator variables
    under OEC 602, 701, and 403. He pointed to many system
    variables and estimator variables that he contended were
    present in the case, and he argued that those “system and
    estimator variables reveal that there are questions about
    the reliability of the identifications sufficient to warrant
    remand for a new hearing and trial, wherein the trial court
    can apply the new Lawson/James test.” In other words,
    7
    That is, we agree that the victim’s identifications were logically relevant
    under OEC 401, and that the state adduced sufficient evidence to support a find-
    ing that the victim had personal knowledge under OEC 602 and that the identi-
    fication was based on the perception of the victim under OEC 701.
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	305
    defendant argued that the probative value of the evidence
    should be determined by considering the relative reliabil-
    ity of the eyewitness testimony, given the system variables
    and the estimator variables, as explained in Lawson/James.
    He argued that the result of the suppression hearing, and,
    therefore, the verdict, could have been different if the court
    had done so, because the identifications were prejudicial
    and facts falling into the categories of system and estimator
    variables cast doubt on their reliability and, therefore, their
    probative value. That, at its core, is an OEC 403 argument.
    We turn, then, to consider whether the presence
    of facts falling into the categories of estimator and system
    variables raises serious questions about the reliability, and,
    thus, the probative value, of the eyewitness identifications.
    If so, then we must consider whether there is little likelihood
    that the error affected the verdict.
    Viewing the facts from defendant’s perspective, a
    trial court applying Lawson/James could find that a num-
    ber of estimator variables were at play that negatively
    affected the victim’s perceptions. To reiterate, at 12:30 a.m.,
    the victim was punched in the side of the head and then
    kicked in the chest or shoulder by one individual, and, as
    he tried to get up, a second person struck him in the head.
    The victim was nearly knocked unconscious and the assail-
    ants then fled. A trial court could find the following facts
    that would support the existence of estimator variables that
    would raise questions about the reliability of the eyewit-
    ness identification. First, the environmental conditions were
    poor, insofar as it was late at night and dark in the park-
    ing lot, and the victim’s encounter with the assailants was
    very brief. Additionally, the victim told a police officer a few
    hours after the assault that he had been “blindsided.” That
    night, he also stated to the officer that he could not recog-
    nize his attackers “specifically, individually,” and he could
    not describe them. On the night of the attack, the victim
    was not sure about having been punched, and he did not
    remember being hit in the head or a hammer being used to
    assault him. Those facts could permit a court to find that
    the victim was under stress because of the surprise nature
    of the attack and because he was badly injured.
    306	                                           State v. Haugen
    The trial court in this case expressly discounted
    those estimator variables in its letter opinion, but, as this
    court explained in Lawson/James, there are recent scientific
    studies that show that poor environmental viewing condi-
    tions, shorter durations of exposure, witness inattention,
    and high levels of stress all can impair a witness’s ability to
    encode memory and undermine the reliability of eyewitness
    identification. Lawson/James, 352 Or at 744-46.
    Similarly, the following facts, viewed in a light favor-
    able to defendant, could support a trial court finding that
    several system variables were present and raise concerns.
    Five days after the assault, the victim told Detective Brown
    that he was attacked by two Vagos members whom he had
    seen earlier in the bar. He told Brown that he was punched
    and kicked by a “great big guy,” about 230 pounds, not fat, in
    his late 20s or early 30s, and that he was hit with a hammer
    by a “little fat guy,” probably in his 40s, with a long pony-
    tail. Brown informed the victim that he would show him
    some photos, and he recited a statement to the effect that
    the perpetrators may or may not be among them. As this
    court explained in Lawson/James, a photo lineup should
    consist of pictures of individuals matching the descriptions
    of the assailants and include among them pictures of sub-
    jects known to the officer to be innocent. However, instead of
    showing the victim pictures of people resembling the “great
    big guy” or the “little fat guy,” Brown showed the victim
    23 photographs of known Vagos motorcycle gang members
    whom Brown suspected either witnessed or participated in
    the assault.
    A trial court also could find that Brown provided
    continuous suggestive and confirming feedback during the
    photo lineup. For example, Detective Brown asked if the big-
    ger man was “pretty buff”; when the victim agreed, Brown
    told the victim that he thought he knew who he was talking
    about. Although Brown later testified that he actually had
    had someone other than defendant in mind, a trial court
    could find that Brown’s comment was nonetheless sugges-
    tive. From the victim’s perspective, Brown had implied that
    defendant’s image would be among the photos shown to him
    and had encouraged the victim to identify someone from
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	307
    that collection. Similarly, Brown implied that the “fat little
    guy” would be among the photos when he told the victim,
    “I’ll show you some photographs—and maybe that will help
    us—once we have some photos we can go through—and we’ll
    identify—who the little fat guy is.” Brown also confirmed the
    identities and gang names of individuals whom the victim
    identified and provided additional information about other
    suspects in the photos, including how their appearances had
    changed since the photos were taken, sometimes before the
    victim himself had commented on the photo. As the court
    stated in Lawson/James, “[w]itness memory can become
    contaminated by external information or assumptions
    embedded in questions or otherwise communicated to the
    witness.” 352 Or at 743. Further, the court stated, “confirm-
    ing feedback tends to falsely inflate witnesses’ confidence in
    the accuracy of their identifications, as well as their recol-
    lections concerning the quality of their opportunity to view
    a perpetrator and an event.” 
    Id. at 744.
    	        Finally, Brown subjected the victim to multiple
    viewings of images of defendant and Rives. After the vic-
    tim completed his review of the initial 23 photos and had
    identified defendant and Rives, Brown told the victim that
    he was going to show him some “more recent photographs
    because there are some significant differences in both the
    gentlemen that you have said that have assaulted you.”
    Based on that conversation, a trial court could find that,
    in other words, Brown told the victim that he was going to
    show him some recent photographs of the individuals whom
    the victim had already identified. Brown then showed the
    victim a series of surveillance photographs that included
    images of defendant, Rives, and others whom the victim had
    identified as having been present during the assault, and
    asked the victim if he could identify anyone in those photo-
    graphs. The victim identified defendant, Rives, and others.
    As the court stated in Lawson/James, “[v]iewing a suspect
    multiple times throughout an investigation can adversely
    affect the reliability of any identification that follows that
    viewing.” 352 Or at 743. That is, even if a witness does not
    select the proper suspect in an initial identification proce-
    dure, the procedure increases the witness’s familiarity with
    that suspect’s face. When the witness later sees the same
    308	                                                          State v. Haugen
    suspect or his or her image again, the suspect tends to stand
    out or appear familiar to the witness. 
    Id. In this
    case, the
    victim became more and more certain that defendant was
    the person who assaulted him as he was shown additional
    pictures of him. But the trial court could find that, in light of
    the scientific research that the court discussed in Lawson/
    James, the victim’s identification actually became less reli-
    able through multiple viewings. 8
    In addition, in Lawson/James, the court identified
    the related problem of “source confusion,” in which a witness
    may be confused or unable to discern the source of his or
    her recognition of the suspect. 352 Or at 743. In Lawson,
    the issue arose because the police suspected that the man
    whom the victim had seen earlier in the day at the camp-
    site was the perpetrator, and, once that idea was implanted
    in the victim’s mind, the court stated, “it would have been
    extremely difficult for [the victim] to mentally separate the
    task of identifying the perpetrator from her brief glimpse of
    his profile in the dark from the task of identifying the man
    she saw earlier in her campsite for about 40 minutes in broad
    daylight.” 
    Id. at 764.
    For similar reasons, a trial court could
    find that that system variable also may be at play in this
    case. As we have stated, immediately after he was attacked,
    the victim was unable to identify his attackers and, in
    fact, stated that he had been blind-sided. A few days later,
    however, the victim identified defendant and Rives as his
    attackers, and he was able to describe defendant and Rives
    in detail and pick out their photos from those that Brown
    showed him. In the circumstance, however, the fact that the
    victim was able to identify defendant and Rives in the pho-
    tos shown to him by Detective Brown does not necessarily
    8
    We note that the trial court found the victim’s identification reliable in part
    because the victim became more and more certain that defendant was one of his
    assailants as he was shown additional pictures of him. The court in Lawson/
    James, however, explained that,
    “[u]nder most circumstances, witness confidence or certainty is not a good
    indicator of identification accuracy. Retrospective self-reports of certainty
    are highly susceptible to suggestive procedures and confirming feedback, a
    factor that further limits the utility of the certainty variable. Witness cer-
    tainty, although a poor indicator of identification accuracy in most cases,
    nevertheless has substantial potential to influence jurors.”
    352 Or at 745.
    Cite as 361 Or 284 (2017)	309
    increase the reliability of his identification of defendant and
    Rives as his attackers. That is so, because, like the victim
    in Lawson, it would have been difficult for the victim in this
    case to mentally separate the task of identifying the perpe-
    trators of the attack, who had blindsided him during a brief
    encounter in a dark parking lot, from the task of identifying
    the people whom he had had ample opportunity to observe
    over the course of the two hours he had spent in the bar with
    his friends, and who had spoken to him as he was leaving
    the bar.
    Relatedly, the trial court (and the Court of Appeals)
    found it compelling that various witnesses “corroborated”
    the victim’s testimony that his assailant was a member
    of the Vagos gang, that the assailant was wearing Vagos
    attire, that the assailant was tall and solidly built, and that
    the assailant was a car salesman. But it is undisputed that
    there were no witnesses to the attack. Thus the “corrobora-
    tion” that the trial court found important to the reliability
    of the identification was not corroboration of the fact that
    defendant was the victim’s assailant; rather, the witnesses
    on whom the trial court relied merely corroborated the vic-
    tim’s memory that defendant had been at the bar.
    In evaluating the admissibility of the eyewitness
    identification evidence in this case, the trial court relied on
    Classen, which this court later held was an incorrect legal
    standard. Defendant, therefore, did not have an opportunity
    to present evidence or expert testimony at the suppression
    hearing explaining any of the estimator variables and the
    system variables that we have just described. And because
    it applied Classen at the suppression hearing, the trial court
    did not have an opportunity to consider them.
    As the court stated in Lawson/James, even when
    the state has met its burden to establish a minimum base-
    line of reliability, “trial courts still must conduct a thorough
    examination of all the pertinent factors in order to deter-
    mine the probative value of the evidence under OEC 403.”
    352 Or at 758. Where an eyewitness has been exposed to
    suggestive police procedures, the trial court has a “height-
    ened role as an evidentiary gatekeeper because ‘traditional’
    methods of testing reliability—like cross-examination—can
    310	                                           State v. Haugen
    be ineffective at discrediting unreliable or inaccurate eye-
    witness identification evidence.” 
    Id. In this
    case, however,
    the court did not have an opportunity to properly exercise
    its gatekeeping role. As we have stated, facts falling into
    the categories of estimator variables and system variables
    in this case raised serious questions about the reliability
    of the identification evidence admitted at defendant’s trial.
    It follows that, as in Lawson, the trial court, not having
    assessed those estimator and system variables, could not
    have made an informed decision about the admissibility of
    the eyewitness identification evidence under OEC 403. We
    conclude, then, that application of the Lawson/James frame-
    work could have resulted in the exclusion of the eyewitness
    identification evidence and, therefore, that the error was not
    harmless.
    Conclusion
    We conclude that the identification procedures used
    in this case raise serious questions about the reliability of the
    victim’s identifications of defendant under Lawson/James.
    Remand, therefore, is necessary to give the trial court the
    opportunity to consider the admissibility of the identifica-
    tions under the correct standard. On remand, at the eviden-
    tiary hearing, the parties must be permitted to supplement
    the record with any additional evidence that may bear on
    the reliability of the eyewitness identification at issue and
    present arguments regarding the appropriate application of
    the procedures set out in Lawson/James. 
    Id. at 765.
    	       The decision of the Court of Appeals is reversed,
    and the case is remanded to the circuit court for further
    proceedings consistent with this decision.
    

Document Info

Docket Number: S063754

Filed Date: 3/30/2017

Precedential Status: Precedential

Modified Date: 4/5/2017