People v. Lerma ( 2016 )


Menu:
  •                           Illinois Official Reports                           Digitally signed by
    Reporter of Decisions
    Reason: I attest to the
    accuracy and integrity of
    this document
    Supreme Court                               Date: 2016.02.29 09:30:46
    -06'00'
    People v. Lerma, 
    2016 IL 118496
    Caption in Supreme   THE PEOPLE OF THE STATE OF ILLINOIS, Appellant, v.
    Court:               EDUARDO LERMA, Appellee.
    Docket No.           118496
    Filed                January 22, 2016
    Decision Under       Appeal from the Appellate Court for the First District; heard in that
    Review               court on appeal from the Circuit Court of Cook County; the Hon.
    Timothy J. Joyce, Judge, presiding.
    Judgment             Appellate court judgment affirmed.
    Counsel on           Lisa Madigan, Attorney General, of Springfield, and Anita Alvarez,
    Appeal               State’s Attorney, of Chicago (Alan J. Spellberg, Michelle Katz and
    Janet C. Mahoney, Assistant State’s Attorneys, of counsel), for the
    People.
    Michael J. Pelletier, State Appellate Defender, Patricia Mysza, Deputy
    Defender, and Linda M. Olthoff, Assistant Appellate Defender, of the
    Office of the State Appellate Defender, of Chicago, for appellee.
    James I. Kaplan, E. King Poor and Daniel B. Lewin, of Quarles &
    Brady LLP, of Chicago, for amicus curiae The Innocence Network.
    Justices                   JUSTICE THOMAS delivered the judgment of the court, with
    opinion.
    Chief Justice Garman and Justices Freeman, Kilbride, Karmeier,
    Burke, and Theis concurred in the judgment and opinion.
    OPINION
    ¶1         The issue is whether, in light of the specific facts and circumstances of this case, the circuit
    court of Cook County abused its discretion when it denied defendant’s motion to allow expert
    testimony concerning the reliability of eyewitness identifications. For the reasons that follow,
    we hold that it did.
    ¶2                                           BACKGROUND
    ¶3          This case arises from the murder of Jason Gill, who in May 2008 was shot to death while
    sitting on the front steps of his Chicago home. The history of this case is set forth fully in the
    appellate court’s opinion below, and we need not repeat the entirety of that history here.
    Instead, we will set forth only those facts pertinent to the issue presently before the court.
    ¶4                                   The Eyewitness Identifications
    ¶5          The evidence of defendant’s guilt consists solely of two eyewitness identifications. The
    first eyewitness identification was made by the victim, Jason Gill, and was admitted into
    evidence under the excited utterance exception to the hearsay rule. 1 The evidence at trial
    established that, on the night of the shooting, Gill was sitting on the front steps of his home
    with a friend, Lydia Clark. At approximately 11:20 p.m., a gunman approached the porch and
    opened fire at Gill and Clark. Gill was struck several times, and Clark dragged Gill into the
    house. Inside the house, Gill’s father, Bill Johnson, asked Gill who had shot him. Both Clark
    and Johnson testified that Gill responded that “Lucky” had shot him. Multiple witnesses,
    including both Clark and Gill’s mother, testified that defendant, who lived across the street
    from Gill, was commonly known by the nickname “Lucky.” In addition, Gill’s mother testified
    that Gill and defendant had been friends for several years, that defendant often spent time in
    Gill’s home, and that defendant recently had been fighting with a member of Gill’s family.
    ¶6          The other eyewitness identification was made by Clark. Clark testified that, on the evening
    of the shooting, she was with Gill on the unlit front steps of Gill’s Chicago home. At
    approximately 11:20 p.m., a man dressed all in black approached Gill’s house, pulled a gun,
    and began shooting at Gill and Clark. Though the man was wearing a hooded sweatshirt, Clark
    1
    The excited utterance exception allows the substantive admission of an otherwise inadmissible
    hearsay statement where the proponent of that statement is able to demonstrate (1) the occurrence of an
    event or condition sufficiently startling to produce a spontaneous and unreflecting statement; (2)
    absence of time to fabricate; and (3) a statement relating to the circumstances of the occurrence. People
    v. Smith, 
    152 Ill. 2d 229
    , 258 (1992). Statements admitted under this exception are admitted because
    such statements “tend to be reliable.” People v. Nevitt, 
    135 Ill. 2d 423
    , 442 (1990).
    -2-
    testified that the hood was down at the time of the shooting.2 Gill covered Clark’s body with
    his, and the two of them fell to the ground together. When the shooting stopped, Clark saw that
    Gill had been shot several times. Clark dragged Gill into the house, where she called 911. By
    this point, Bill Johnson had come downstairs and was asking Gill what had happened. Clark
    heard Gill say, “Lucky shot me.” When the police arrived at the scene, Clark told them that
    “Lucky” had shot Gill. The next morning, at 1:25 a.m., Clark went to the police station, where
    she was shown a photo lineup of six Hispanic males, one of whom was defendant. From the
    photographs, Clark identified defendant as the shooter. One day later, in a one-person
    show-up, Clark again identified defendant as the shooter. In open court, and no less than five
    times, Clark pointed to and specifically identified defendant as the man who shot Gill. Clark
    testified that she knew defendant only by his nickname, “Lucky.” On direct examination, Clark
    testified that, in the six months to a year before the shooting, she had seen defendant on the
    porch across the street from Gill’s house approximately ten times. On cross-examination,
    however, Clark admitted that, in her grand jury testimony, she testified that she had seen
    defendant only “[l]ike once or twice” before the shooting. Either way, Clark had never talked
    to or had a conversation with defendant, and she had never been in the same room or the same
    house with defendant. When asked directly how long she had known defendant prior to the
    shooting, Clark responded, “I did not know him.”
    ¶7                                   Defendant’s Motion in Limine
    ¶8        In anticipation of these eyewitness identifications, defendant filed a pretrial motion
    in limine to allow Dr. Solomon Fulero, an attorney and licensed psychologist, to testify as an
    expert on the topic of memory and eyewitness identification. Defendant argued that
    Dr. Fulero’s testimony would aid the jury by identifying and explaining several “common
    misperceptions” that exist concerning the accuracy and reliability of eyewitness
    identifications. According to defendant’s motion, which included a report authored by
    Dr. Fulero, Dr. Fulero’s testimony would include the following scientifically documented
    findings, all of which are beyond the common knowledge of the average layperson: that the
    witness’s level of confidence does not necessarily correlate to the accuracy of the
    identification; that numerous factors can undermine the accuracy of an eyewitness’s
    identification, including the stress of the event itself, the presence of a weapon, the passage of
    time, the “forgetting curve,” the wearing of partial disguises such as hoods, exposure to
    postevent information, nighttime viewing, and suggestive police identification procedures;
    that eyewitnesses tend to overestimate time frames; and that cross-racial identifications tend to
    be less reliable than same-race identifications.
    ¶9        The State opposed defendant’s motion on three principle grounds. First, citing this court’s
    decision in People v. Enis, 
    139 Ill. 2d 264
    (1990), and the First District’s decision in People v.
    Tisdel, 
    316 Ill. App. 3d 1143
    (2000), the State argued that “Illinois courts have consistently
    upheld a trial court’s decision to bar expert testimony regarding witness identification.”
    Second, the State argued that the matters about which Dr. Fulero intended to testify are well
    within the common knowledge of the average layperson and therefore could be addressed
    adequately through cross-examination, closing arguments, and jury instructions. Third, the
    2
    Detective Michael Hughes, who interviewed Clark after the shooting, testified that Clark reported
    on the night of the shooting that the assailant’s hood was up.
    -3-
    State argued that the data and conclusions contained in Dr. Fulero’s report “do not fit the facts
    of this case” because they deal solely with stranger identifications, whereas in this case both
    Gill and Clark knew defendant prior to the crime. To bolster this point, the State presented the
    trial court with an unpublished decision from the Ohio Court of Appeals containing a summary
    description of Dr. Fulero’s testimony from a 1999 murder trial. See State v. Nickleberry, No.
    77516, 
    2000 WL 1738356
    , at *3 (Ohio Ct. App. Nov. 22, 2000). According to that summary,
    while Dr. Fulero “testified that eyewitness identification may be unreliable because of a
    variety of factors,” he also “admitted *** that these factors are considered applicable where the
    eyewitness is viewing a stranger and not someone he or she has met before.” 
    Id. In addition,
    the
    State presented the trial court with an article that it “pulled off the internet yesterday,” in which
    a “Mr. Mark Green who has a Ph.D. in psychology” wrote that “[t]here are some situations
    where identification is more likely accurate,” “[f]or example, if the suspect is someone
    previously known to the victim, then high accuracy is more probable.”
    ¶ 10        Following oral arguments, the trial court denied defendant’s motion to allow Dr. Fulero’s
    expert witness testimony on the reliability of eyewitness identifications. In so ruling, the trial
    court emphasized that the one fact that distinguishes this case from other identification cases is
    that the eyewitnesses here knew defendant prior to the shooting. According to the trial court:
    “[I]t is not a circumstance that requires the testimony of an expert to establish what
    pretty much everybody knows, which *** it is a fact that persons *** are less likely to
    misidentify someone they have met or know or seen before than a stranger. That’s not a
    function of psychology or expert opinion testimony. It is a function of human nature,
    and it is not something that would require the application of expert opinion testimony
    because it is not beyond the ken of an ordinary juror.”
    In addition, the trial court reasoned that, because the eyewitnesses in this case knew defendant
    prior to the crime, “the claims Dr. Fulero speaks to regarding cross-racial difficulties, certainty
    or confidence, stress, weapons focus, multiple witness identifications, suggestibility, they are
    necessarily made less relevant than they would be if these persons had never seen the shooter
    before the night in question.” The trial court then explained that this decrease in relevance
    “leads to a concomitant increase of potential prejudice at least in the relative sense.” More
    specifically, the trial court explained that, given its relative lack of relevance, Dr. Fulero’s
    testimony ran the risk of both “generat[ing] *** a referendum on the efficacy of identification
    testimony generally” and “operating as his opinion on the credibility” of the eyewitnesses.
    ¶ 11                             Defendant’s First Motion to Reconsider
    ¶ 12       One month later, and before his trial began, defendant filed a brief motion to reconsider the
    denial of his motion in limine. In support, defendant cited and attached the New Jersey
    Supreme Court’s then-recent decision in State v. Henderson, 
    27 A.3d 872
    (N.J. 2011), which
    comprehensively reviewed the current state of scientific research concerning the reliability of
    eyewitness identifications. In addition, at the hearing on the motion to reconsider, defense
    counsel informed the trial court that, if the motion was allowed, Dr. Fulero would “testify that
    misidentifications have occurred with people who the witness knew beforehand.” At the
    conclusion of the hearing, the trial court denied defendant’s motion to reconsider. In doing so,
    the trial court began by observing that “in the years that I have been doing this in one capacity
    or another there is always an issue or two du jour” and that “this seems to be one that is coming
    -4-
    on strong.” From there, the trial court explained that it was denying the motion to reconsider
    “for the reasons previously expressed at some considerable length,” the “glaring one” of which
    is that “the persons who identify Mr. Lerma *** all claim to have known him.” According to
    the trial court, “[t]hat factor makes this [case] sufficiently distinct from those cases where
    identification procedures are of special moment and subject to the dangers which arise when
    strangers are called upon to identify persons [who are] also strangers.” The trial court then
    pointed out that its decision was “supported by the defense’s own witness, Dr. Fulero,” who is
    reported to have offered a similar opinion in Nickleberry.3
    ¶ 13                            Defendant’s Second Motion to Reconsider
    ¶ 14       Midway through trial, and after the State had presented the eyewitness testimony set forth
    above, defense counsel renewed defendant’s motion to call an eyewitness identification expert.
    Because Dr. Fulero had since passed away, defense counsel this time tendered to the trial court
    a report authored by Dr. Geoffrey Loftus, a professor of psychology at the University of
    Washington and widely published and globally recognized expert in the field of human
    perception and memory. The data and conclusions contained in Dr. Loftus’s report largely
    tracked with the contents of Dr. Fulero’s report, with two significant exceptions. First,
    Dr. Loftus’s report stressed that he would not “issue judgments about whether a particular
    witness’s memory and assertions *** are correct or incorrect” and that “any testimony on [his]
    part which implies unreliability on the part of eyewitness(es) who identify a defendant should
    not, ipso facto, be taken to imply that the defendant is innocent—it implies only that the
    eyewitness evidence should be viewed with appropriate caution.” Second, and more
    importantly, unlike Dr. Fulero’s report, which was silent on the subject of acquaintance
    identifications, Dr. Loftus’s report specifically stated that “[i]t would seem intuitive to a jury
    that if a witness identifies a suspect with whom he or she is acquainted, the witness’s
    identification would likely be accurate. However, this is not necessarily true.” Rather, the
    report explained, “if circumstances are poor for a witness’s ability to perceive a person,” and
    “the situation fosters a witness’s expectations that he or she will see a particular
    acquaintance[,] *** then the witness will tend to perceive the person as the expected
    acquaintance even if the person is in fact someone else.” According to Dr. Loftus’s report, such
    poor circumstances include low lighting; viewing longer distances in the dark; divided
    attention of the witness, including a focus on a weapon; time duration, with less time leading to
    less available information, and a witness’s tendency to overestimate time durations;
    cross-racial identification; stress; and a partially obscured face. Dr. Loftus stated such
    situations may lead to misidentification because:
    “In such circumstances, the witness’s acquaintance with the expected—and hence
    perceived—person works against accurate identification for two reasons: First, it
    would be natural and easy for the witness to subsequently pick the acquaintance in an
    identification procedure *** (because the witness already knows whom she is seeking
    in a lineup procedure, she could immediately rule out all the fillers, and zero in on the
    3
    In making this observation, the trial court acknowledged that Dr. Fulero in fact disputed the
    accuracy of Nickleberry’s description of his testimony in that case. Nevertheless, the trial court stated
    that “when an appellate court justice acting on the record in an Ohio appeals court case claims that
    [Dr. Fulero] did in fact [state that opinion], I am not going any further down that road.”
    -5-
    acquaintance/suspect). Second, the witness could use his or her prior knowledge of the
    acquaintance’s appearance to reconstruct his or her memory of the original events—the
    crime—such that the in fact poor original memory of the actual criminal is replaced
    with a stronger and more confidence-evoking memory of the acquaintance ***.”
    ¶ 15       In response to defendant’s renewed motion, the State argued simply that “[n]othing has
    changed from the previous situation when you heard these motions.” Rather, the State argued:
    “[Clark] still knows the defendant, still always maintained she knew him, and as we
    discussed before these studies and these issues and expert testimony on identification
    issue has primarily focused on individuals who did not know the person that they were
    identifying prior to the incident that occurred. Nothing with regard to that has
    changed.”
    ¶ 16       After hearing from both sides, the trial court stated that it was denying defendant’s motion
    to present Dr. Loftus’s testimony “consistent with the reasons [the court] set forth in detail
    when [the court] made the ruling on your similar motion with respect to Dr. Fulero.”
    ¶ 17                                     Conviction and Sentence
    ¶ 18       In the end, the jury convicted defendant of first degree murder, personally discharging the
    firearm that caused Jason Gill’s death, and aggravated discharge of a weapon. The trial court
    sentenced defendant to 45 years in prison.
    ¶ 19                                        Defendant’s Appeal
    ¶ 20        Defendant appealed, and the appellate court reversed and remanded. 
    2014 IL App (1st) 121880
    . In doing so, the appellate court reasoned that, under this court’s decision in People v.
    Enis, 
    139 Ill. 2d 264
    (1990), when deciding whether to admit expert testimony on the subject
    of eyewitness identification, a trial court is required to “carefully consider the necessity and
    relevance of the expert testimony in light of the facts in the case before him.” 
    Id. at 290;
    see
    also People v. Tisdel, 
    338 Ill. App. 3d 465
    , 468 (2003) (holding that “[t]rial courts should
    carefully scrutinize the proffered testimony to determine its relevance—that is, whether there
    is a logical connection between the testimony and the facts of the case”). According to the
    appellate court, the trial court here clearly failed to do this, as the reasons the trial court gave
    for denying the admission of Dr. Fulero’s testimony (i.e., that “it is a fact” that acquaintance
    identifications are reliable and that expert testimony in this case would be “operating as ***
    opinion on the credibility” of the eyewitnesses) could not possibly serve as a basis for denying
    the admission of Dr. Loftus’s testimony, as Dr. Loftus’s report directly refutes both of these
    points. 
    2014 IL App (1st) 121880
    , ¶¶ 36, 38. Nevertheless, in denying the admission of
    Dr. Loftus’s testimony, the trial court stated that its ruling was for reasons “consistent with the
    reasons” it gave for denying the admission of Dr. Fulero’s testimony. According to the
    appellate court, this demonstrates that “the trial court *** did not carefully consider or
    scrutinize Dr. Loftus’s report where [that] report directly contradicted the court’s prior finding
    that it is common knowledge that an eyewitness is less likely to misidentify an acquaintance.”
    
    Id. ¶ 36.
    Moreover, the appellate court explained, the trial court’s initial ruling denying the
    admission of Dr. Fulero’s testimony was itself an abuse of discretion because the reasons the
    trial court gave for that ruling amounted to “little more than a series of conclusions based on its
    personal belief” that acquaintance identifications are accurate and therefore not a proper
    -6-
    subject for expert testimony. 
    Id. ¶ 38.
    Thus, when it later invoked those same reasons as the
    basis for its decision denying the admission of Dr. Loftus’s testimony, the trial court was not
    only invoking reasons that were now factually inapposite but also reasserting its own
    “subjective value judgments” as the basis for rejecting the considered opinion of a qualified
    expert. 
    Id. Accordingly, the
    appellate court reversed the trial court’s ruling denying the
    admission of Dr. Loftus’s testimony and remanded the cause for a new trial that includes the
    admission of expert testimony on the matter of eyewitness identification. 
    Id. ¶ 40.
    ¶ 21       The State appealed to this court, and we allowed its petition for leave to appeal. Ill. S. Ct. R.
    315 (eff. July 1, 2013).
    ¶ 22                                             ANALYSIS
    ¶ 23        A criminal defendant’s right to due process and a fundamentally fair trial includes the right
    to present witnesses on his or her own behalf. People v. Wheeler, 
    151 Ill. 2d 298
    , 305 (1992).
    “In Illinois, generally, an individual will be permitted to testify as an expert if his experience
    and qualifications afford him knowledge which is not common to lay persons and where such
    testimony will aid the trier of fact in reaching its conclusion.” 
    Enis, 139 Ill. 2d at 288
    . In
    addressing the admission of expert testimony, the trial court should balance the probative value
    of the evidence against its prejudicial effect to determine the reliability of the testimony. 
    Id. at 290.
    In addition, in the exercise of its discretion, the trial court should carefully consider the
    necessity and relevance of the expert testimony in light of the particular facts of the case before
    admitting that testimony for the jury’s consideration. 
    Id. This court
    has held that expert
    testimony is only necessary when the subject is both particularly within the witness’s
    experience and qualifications and beyond that of the average juror’s, and when it will aid the
    jury in reaching its conclusion. People v. Cloutier, 
    156 Ill. 2d 483
    , 501 (1993). Expert
    testimony addressing matters of common knowledge is not admissible “unless the subject is
    difficult to understand and explain.” People v. Becker, 
    239 Ill. 2d 215
    , 235 (2010). When
    determining the reliability of an expert witness, a trial court is given broad discretion. 
    Enis, 139 Ill. 2d at 290
    . Therefore, we review the trial court’s decision to admit evidence, including
    expert witness testimony, for an abuse of that discretion. 
    Becker, 239 Ill. 2d at 234
    . An abuse of
    discretion occurs only where the trial court’s decision is “arbitrary, fanciful, or unreasonable to
    the degree that no reasonable person would agree with it.” People v. Rivera, 
    2013 IL 112467
    ,
    ¶ 37.
    ¶ 24        The issue in this case is whether the trial court abused its discretion in denying defendant’s
    request to allow Dr. Loftus’s expert testimony on the reliability of eyewitness identifications.
    Before addressing that question directly, we would like briefly to comment on the current state
    of jurisprudence concerning the admission of eyewitness expert testimony, which contrary to
    the trial court’s belief, cannot possibly be dismissed as a mere “issue du jour.” The last time
    this court addressed the admission of such testimony was in Enis, which was decided more
    than 25 years ago when the relevant research was in its relative infancy. Even then, this court
    recognized that “in the past decade a number of courts have held that expert testimony
    concerning eyewitness identification should be admissible in certain circumstances.” 
    Enis, 139 Ill. 2d at 286-87
    (collecting cases). Nevertheless, this court also expressed skepticism and
    caution against the overuse of such testimony (id. at 289), such that the exclusion of such
    testimony remains the common practice in Illinois to this day. See, e.g., People v. McGhee,
    -7-
    2012 IL App (1st) 093404, ¶ 55 (observing that “Illinois continues to reject, at least in practice,
    expert testimony on the reliability of eyewitnesses”). The decades since Enis, however, have
    seen a dramatic shift in the legal landscape, as expert testimony concerning the reliability of
    eyewitness testimony has moved from novel and uncertain to settled and widely accepted.
    Indeed, as the Supreme Court of Pennsylvania recently noted, there is now “a clear trend
    among state and federal courts permitting the admission of eyewitness expert testimony, at the
    discretion of the trial court, for the purpose of aiding the trier of fact in understanding the
    characteristics of eyewitness identification.” Commonwealth v. Walker, 
    92 A.3d 766
    , 782-83
    (Pa. 2014) (collecting demonstrative cases from 44 states, the District of Columbia, and 10
    federal circuit courts). The reason for this trend is that, although findings of the sort described
    in Dr. Fulero’s and Dr. Loftus’s reports are now “widely accepted by scientists,” those same
    findings “are largely unfamiliar to the average person, and, in fact, many of the findings are
    counterintuitive.” State v. Guilbert, 
    49 A.3d 705
    , 723-24 (Conn. 2012) (collecting cases and
    studies demonstrating this point). At the same time, advances in DNA testing have confirmed
    that “eyewitness misidentification is now the single greatest source of wrongful convictions in
    the United States, and responsible for more wrongful convictions than all other causes
    combined.” State v. Dubose, 
    699 N.W.2d 582
    , 591-92 (Wis. 2005) (collecting relevant
    studies). In other words, in the 25 years since Enis, we not only have seen that eyewitness
    identifications are not always as reliable as they appear, but we also have learned, from a
    scientific standpoint, why this is often the case. Accordingly, whereas Enis allowed for but
    expressed caution toward the developing research concerning eyewitness identifications, today
    we are able to recognize that such research is well settled, well supported, and in appropriate
    cases a perfectly proper subject for expert testimony.
    ¶ 25        With that in mind, we now turn to the issue before us: whether, given the specific facts
    presented, the trial court abused its discretion in denying defendant’s request to allow
    Dr. Loftus’s expert testimony in this case. For the following reasons, we hold that it did.
    ¶ 26        To begin with, there is no question that this is the type of case for which expert eyewitness
    testimony is both relevant and appropriate. The only evidence of defendant’s guilt in this case
    is the eyewitness identifications made by Clark and Gill. There is no physical evidence tying
    defendant to the crime, and defendant neither confessed nor made any other type of
    incriminating statement. In other words, the State’s case against defendant hangs 100% on the
    reliability of its eyewitness identifications. In addition, of the several factors that both
    Dr. Fulero and Dr. Loftus identified as potentially contributing to the unreliability of
    eyewitness testimony, most are either present or possibly present in this case. These include
    the stress of the event itself, the use and presence of a weapon, the wearing of a partial disguise,
    exposure to postevent information, nighttime viewing, and cross-racial identification.4 Next,
    of the State’s two eyewitness identifications, only one was subject to adversarial testing and
    cross-examination at trial, as Gill’s identification was admitted through the testimony of other
    witnesses under the excited utterance exception to the hearsay rule. And finally, while both the
    trial court and the State insist that both Gill and Clark “knew” defendant prior to the crime and
    that their identifications of defendant therefore are presumptively reliable, the record is far
    from clear on this point as it relates to Clark. Indeed, while Clark testified at trial that she had
    seen defendant from across the street approximately ten times in the six months to a year prior
    4
    The record shows that Clark is African-American, whereas defendant is Hispanic.
    -8-
    to the shooting, she testified before the grand jury that she had seen him only “[l]ike once or
    twice” before the shooting. Either way, Clark had only ever seen him standing on a porch from
    across the street. She expressly testified that she had never spoken with him before, had never
    been in the same room with him, and had never even been in the same house as him before.
    And when asked directly how long she had known defendant prior to the shooting, Clark’s
    response was, “I did not know him.” Even construed in the State’s favor, this degree of
    “knowledge” is limited at best and nothing like the State’s representation to the trial court that
    Clark “knows the defendant” and “always maintained she knew him.” Again, Clark expressly
    denied knowing him. Taken together, these considerations convince us that this is exactly the
    type of case in which expert eyewitness testimony would be both probative and admissible.
    ¶ 27        The next question, therefore, is whether the trial court abused its discretion in denying
    defendant’s request to admit Dr. Loftus’s expert testimony in this case. We hold that it did. The
    trial court denied defendant’s request for reasons “consistent with the reasons [it] set forth in
    detail when [it] made the ruling on [defendant’s] similar motion with respect to Dr. Fulero.” As
    discussed above, those reasons consisted primarily of the trial court’s personal conviction that
    “it is a fact that persons *** are less likely to misidentify someone they have met or know or
    seen before than a stranger,” as well as the trial court’s belief that expert testimony of this sort
    would both “generate *** a referendum on the efficacy of identification testimony generally”
    and “operat[e] as [an] opinion on the credibility” of the eyewitnesses themselves. As to the first
    of those points, the trial court emphasized at the hearing on defendant’s first motion to
    reconsider that its reasoning was “supported by the defense’s own witness, Dr. Fulero,” who is
    reported to have offered the same opinion in Nickleberry.
    ¶ 28        The problem with the trial court’s reasoning is that, even if it is defensible as to
    Dr. Fulero’s expected testimony, it is not defensible as to Dr. Loftus’s expected testimony. To
    be sure, Dr. Fulero’s report did not specifically address identifications made by witnesses who
    were acquainted with the accused prior to the crime. Neither did Dr. Fulero’s report
    specifically address the scope of his testimony and whether he would comment or offer an
    opinion on the credibility of Clark’s identification in this case. But Dr. Loftus’s report did
    address both of these matters directly, and it contradicted both of the trial court’s previous
    assumptions about what Dr. Fulero would say on these points. Again, after reading
    Dr. Fulero’s report, the trial court concluded that expert eyewitness testimony was unnecessary
    in this case both because “everyone knows” that acquaintance identifications are reliable and
    because Dr. Fulero’s testimony would likely “operat[e] as [an] opinion on the credibility” of
    the eyewitnesses themselves. Dr. Loftus’s report, however, directly addressed both of these
    points and stated the very opposite of what the trial court had previously assumed. According
    to Dr. Loftus’s report, the factors impacting the reliability of eyewitness identifications can
    operate even when the witness is previously acquainted with the accused, and Dr. Loftus’s
    testimony would not include any opinion as to the credibility of any specific witness or any
    specific identification. Nevertheless, in denying defendant’s request to allow Dr. Loftus’s
    testimony, the trial court simply invoked the reasons it gave for denying the admission of
    Dr. Fulero’s testimony, neither of which had any continued relevance in light of Dr. Loftus’s
    report. Indeed, Dr. Loftus’s report flatly contradicted the trial court’s understanding. At this
    point, it is important to reiterate that, even in Enis, this court recognized that eyewitness
    identification is an appropriate subject for expert testimony. As importantly, both the State and
    the trial court in this case concede that Dr. Loftus is a qualified and highly respected expert in
    -9-
    this field. Thus, in relying on its own personal beliefs about how eyewitness identifications
    function as the primary basis for denying the admission of Dr. Loftus’s testimony, the trial
    court in this case not only ignored the explicit contents of Dr. Loftus’s report but also
    effectively substituted its own opinion on a matter of uncommon knowledge for that of a
    respected and qualified expert.
    ¶ 29       At the same time, the trial court’s repeated insistence that the eyewitnesses in this case both
    “knew” defendant prior to the shooting is not nearly as certain and settled a fact as the trial
    court suggests. To be sure, there is no question that Gill knew defendant well and for several
    years prior to the shooting. As to Clark, however, the evidence is much less clear. Again,
    although Clark testified at trial that she had seen defendant from across the street
    approximately 10 times in the six months to a year prior to the shooting, she testified before the
    grand jury that she had seen him only “[l]ike once or twice” before the shooting. Either way,
    Clark had only ever seen him standing on a porch from across the street. She expressly testified
    that she had never spoken with him before, had never been in the same room with him before,
    and had never even been in the same house as him before. And when asked directly how long
    she had known defendant prior to the shooting, Clark’s response was, “I did not know him.”
    Depending, then, upon which version of Clark’s testimony the jury chose to believe, the State’s
    only testifying eyewitness in this case is someone who had seen defendant from across the
    street possibly once but no more than 10 times in the year before the shooting, had never
    spoken to defendant, had never been in the same room or in the same house as defendant, and
    expressly denied knowing defendant. Nevertheless, the trial court’s primary basis for denying
    defendant’s request to present expert eyewitness testimony in this case is the “glaring” fact that
    “the persons who identify Mr. Lerma *** all claim to have known him.” This is overstating the
    State’s evidence at best.
    ¶ 30       Finally, we note that, in denying defendant’s motion, the trial court expressly invoked the
    Ohio Court of Appeals’ decision in Nickleberry, claiming that the summary of Dr. Fulero’s
    testimony in that decision demonstrates that the trial court’s ruling was “supported by the
    defense’s own witness.” There are at least two problems with this. The first problem is that
    defense counsel informed the trial court on multiple occasions that the one-sentence summary
    contained in Nickleberry does not accurately reflect Dr. Fulero’s full testimony in that case and
    that Dr. Fulero was fully prepared to clarify that if allowed to testify. In addition, defense
    counsel informed the trial court that Dr. Fulero was prepared to testify specifically in
    defendant’s case that “misidentifications have occurred with people who the witness knew
    beforehand.” Rather than hear from Dr. Fulero himself, however, the trial court chose to treat
    Nickleberry’s one-sentence summary of Dr. Fulero’s 1999 testimony not only as indisputably
    accurate but also as a binding and authoritative representation of Dr. Fulero’s opinion in 2012,
    such that the trial court cited Nickleberry as a valid basis for denying defendant’s motion.
    Needless to say, we find this troubling, as it is not difficult to imagine either that a
    one-sentence summary fails to capture the full complexity of an expert witness’s entire trial
    testimony, or that an expert opinion might have evolved over the course of 13 years.
    Unfortunately, the trial court’s approach foreclosed the discovery of either possibility.
    ¶ 31       The second problem with the trial court’s invocation of Nickleberry is that, even assuming
    that the one-sentence summary of Dr. Fulero’s testimony in that case is accurate, that summary
    in no way supports the trial court’s decision to exclude expert testimony in this case.
    According to the trial court, Dr Fulero’s testimony, as summarized in Nickleberry, supports the
    - 10 -
    trial court’s belief that the factors impacting the reliability of eyewitness identifications are not
    present in a case such as this, where “the persons who identify [the defendant] *** all claim to
    have known him.” (Emphasis added.) Likewise, in defending the trial court’s decision before
    this court, the State points out that when the trial court denied defendant’s request to present
    Dr. Loftus’s expert testimony, it was aware that Dr. Loftus’s claim that “misidentification can
    occur when a witness knows the subject” was “directly contradicted by” Dr. Fulero’s
    testimony, as summarized in Nickleberry. (Emphasis added.) The problem with both the trial
    court’s and the State’s positions (aside from the fact that Clark expressly disavowed knowing
    defendant), is that Nickleberry’s summary of Dr. Fulero’s testimony says absolutely nothing
    about when a witness “knows” the person he or she is identifying. On the contrary, according
    to that summary, Dr. Fulero testified in Nickleberry that the factors impacting the reliability of
    eyewitness identifications are not present when the witness is identifying “someone he or she
    has met before.” (Emphasis added.) Nickleberry, 
    2000 WL 1738356
    , at *3. This distinction is
    very important because, in this case, Clark’s testimony was unequivocal that she had never met
    defendant prior to the shooting. Again, Clark denied ever having spoken to defendant, ever
    having been in same room or house as defendant, and for that matter even “knowing”
    defendant. Given Clark’s testimony on this point, Dr. Fulero’s prior opinion that
    identifications are more reliable when the witness has “met” the accused is not only irrelevant
    to the actual facts of this case, it cannot possibly support the trial court’s decision denying
    defendant’s request to present expert testimony on the reliability of eyewitness identifications.
    Nevertheless, according to both the trial court and the State, Dr. Fulero’s prior opinion did
    exactly that, serving as one of the primary justifications for the exclusion of both Dr. Fulero’s
    and Dr. Loftus’s testimony in this case.
    ¶ 32        Abuse of discretion is a highly deferential standard of review, and we reiterate that we will
    find such abuse only when the trial court’s decision is “arbitrary, fanciful, or unreasonable to
    the degree that no reasonable person would agree with it.” Rivera, 
    2013 IL 112467
    , ¶ 37. We
    find that to be the case here. As discussed above, what we have in this case is the trial court
    denying defendant’s request to present relevant and probative testimony from a qualified
    expert that speaks directly to the State’s only evidence against him, and doing so for reasons
    that are both expressly contradicted by the expert’s report and inconsistent with the actual facts
    of the case. A decision of that nature rises to the level of both arbitrary and unreasonable to an
    unacceptable degree, and we therefore find that the trial court’s decision denying defendant’s
    request to admit Dr. Loftus’s expert testimony was an abuse of discretion.
    ¶ 33        The only remaining question is whether the trial court’s error was harmless. We hold that it
    was not. This court has recognized three approaches to determine whether an error such as this
    is harmless beyond a reasonable doubt: (1) whether the error contributed to the defendant’s
    conviction; (2) whether the other evidence in the case overwhelmingly supported the
    defendant’s conviction; and (3) whether the excluded evidence would have been duplicative or
    cumulative. People v. Blue, 
    205 Ill. 2d 1
    , 26 (2001). In this case, each of these approaches
    establishes that the trial court’s decision excluding Dr. Loftus’s testimony was not harmless
    beyond a reasonable doubt. First, there is no question that the error contributed to the
    defendant’s conviction, as the exclusion of Dr. Loftus’s testimony prevented the jury from
    hearing relevant and probative expert testimony relating to the State’s sole testifying
    eyewitness, in a case lacking any physical evidence linking defendant to the crime. Second, it
    cannot be said that the other evidence in the case overwhelmingly supported the defendant’s
    - 11 -
    conviction, as other than Clark’s testimony, the only evidence of defendant’s guilt is a hearsay
    excited utterance from a nontestifying witness. And while such evidence is certainly sufficient
    to support defendant’s conviction, we cannot say that it does so “overwhelmingly.” Third, the
    excluded testimony from Dr. Loftus was neither duplicative nor cumulative of other evidence,
    as the jury in this case heard precisely nothing in the nature of expert eyewitness testimony.
    ¶ 34                                         CONCLUSION
    ¶ 35        For the foregoing reasons, we affirm the judgment of the appellate court, which reversed
    the judgment of the trial court and remanded the cause for a new trial with directions to allow
    expert testimony on eyewitness identification subject to the provisions of Rule 702 of the
    Illinois Rules of Evidence (eff. Jan. 1, 2011). In doing so, we reiterate that the totality of the
    evidence presented at defendant’s trial was sufficient to prove defendant’s guilt beyond a
    reasonable doubt, such that no double jeopardy violation will occur on retrial. People v. Ward,
    
    2011 IL 108690
    , ¶ 50.
    ¶ 36      Appellate court judgment affirmed.
    - 12 -